Skip to main content

Full text of "Heredity in the light of recent research"

See other formats


HEREDITY 
| IN THE LIGHT OF (8 | 
RECENT RESEARCH [2 


L.DONCASTER 


r 
= 
ual 
: 3 
z ¥ 4 
> 2; * > 
; . » ~s 
— at 3 1 
~~ eB x : 
A = Ze ats — 
=: 
= aoe 
== 
SS 
~- a . 
Sie 25 
x 2 3 
AAS my 
Ag : + : 
B — 
‘ 
> 


wi UG 
Hates ; a ‘ as 4 
ia 


The Cambridge Manuals of Science and 
Literature 


SUE IBIBIAY 


CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSIDZY BRESS 
Hondon: FETTER LANE, E.C. 
C. F. CLAY, MANAGER 


Evinburgh: 100, PRINCES STREET 
Berlin: A. ASHER AND CO. 
Letpsig: F. A. BROCKHAUS 
few Work: G. P. PUTNAM’S SONS 
Bombay and Calcutta: MACMILLAN AND CO., Lrp. 


All rights reserved 


TTT Pros 


Om 
aS 


SH 
yy 


a 
L We 
LA 
Py ‘ 
"“”* = 
- : 


Al 
zea 
Pde 
WY’ 


« 
2° 
Cx 
SS 


< 
> = 
ie 
= cl 
ueeet 
NM 

2, a. 
ca Z = 
op Ore 
an 


ae 


CANN DAU 


Cambridge: 
PRINEKD BY JOHN. CLAY, M.A. 
AT THE UNIVERSIVY PRESS 


With the exception of the coat of arms at 


the foot, the design on the title page is a 


reproduction of one used by the earhest known 
Cambridge printer John Siberch 1521 


PREFACE 


ie a book of the size to which the Cambridge 
Manuals of Science and Literature are limited, it 
- is plainly impossible to treat in detail every aspect of 
a subject like Heredity. One of the chief difficulties, 
therefore, which I have encountered in preparing this 
little book has been to decide what to leave out. To 
some it will doubtless seem that parts of the subject 
have been treated too fully, and other important 
branches omitted or barely mentioned, but my aim 
has been to give the reader a sketch of the most 
important lines in which recent advances have been 
made. There are many excellent works dealing with 
the older theories—and in this subject age is measured 
by very few years,—but our knowledge has increased 
so greatly and is still progressing so quickly that 
books become out of date almost as soon as they are 
published. My attempt, then, has been to deal chiefly 
with the quite modern developments of the subject, 
and in order that the reader who is not very familiar 
with the matter may feel he is on fairly sure ground, 


vi PREFACE 


and not confuse fact with speculation, I have tried to 
avoid purely speculative questions in the body of the 
book, and have devoted a few pages to one of the most 
interesting of these in an appendix, together with a 
historical summary of Theories of Heredity. There 
are many, of course, who will regard parts of the 
chapters dealing with Mendelism as consisting largely 
of speculation ; I can only reply that I regard the 
facts referred to as established, and the theoretical 
deductions from them as the only ones that have 
yet been offered which can fit them adequately. 

No attempt has been made to quote authorities 
for every statement, but a list of books and papers is 
given in which a further account will be found of the 
subjects treated. The numbers in square brackets | | 
in the text refer to this list. I have also followed 
the example set by Mr Lock in providing a glossary 
of unfamiliar terms of which the use has been 
unavoidable. 

For the chapter on Statistical Study of Inherit- 
ance my chief sources of information have been 
Prof. Pearson’s Grammar of Science and his numerous 
papers on the subject published by the Royal Society 
and in Biometrika. I have tried to summarise in 
words the results of his work, and of that of other 
workers on similar lines, and if the inadequacy of my 
mathematical knowledge has led me into any serious 
errors in the attempt, I owe them my apologies. 


PREFACE vil 


Chiefly of course I am indebted to Prof. Bateson’s 
recent work on Mendel’s Principles of Heredity as 
the most complete and authoritative account of the 
subject, from the acknowledged leader of the Men- 
delian school. For permission to reproduce several 
figures from this book I tender my thanks. In 
addition to information from many original papers, I 
have also not hesitated to make use of Lock’s Recent 
Progress in the Study of Variation, Heredity and 
Kvolution, Thomson’s Heredity, and some other 
books dealing with the general aspects of the subject; 
to these authors, and to several friends who have 
been kind enough to give me written information 
on matters with which they are especially conversant, 
I wish to record my indebtedness. I wish also 
especially to thank Mrs A. C. Seward for drawing 
the sections of Primula reproduced in fig. 10 


(p. 81). 
L. DONCASTER. 


CAMBRIDGE, 
June 1910. 


CHAP. 


UE 


IVE 


ale 


CONTENTS 


Introduction. Relation of Heredity to other branches of 
knowledge.-—The questions to be answered. page 1 


Variation. Occurrence and kinds of Variation.—Con- 
tinuous variation and methods of study.— Discontinuous 
variation.—Inborn and acquired characters i 


The Causes of Variation. Mutation.—Action of en- 
vironment on body and germ-cells.—Variation on cross- 
ing.— Relative importance of ‘inherent’ and ‘acquired’ 
characters. : : : : 22 


The Statistical Study of Heredity. Two methods of 
studying Heredity.—The biometrical method.—Corre- 
lation and Regression.—Parental correlation and the 
Law of Ancestral Heredity.—Heredity in ‘pure lines’ 
(Johannsen ete.).—Inheritance of ‘mental and moral’ 
characters in Man.—Eugenics. 4 4 32 


Mendelian Heredity. Mendel’s Law illustrated.—Segre- 
gation and Allelomorphism.—Crosses concerning more 
than one pair of characters.—Hxamples of Mendelian 
characters in Plants and Animals.—Combs of Fowls.— 
The Andalusian Fowl . : : 52 


. 
_ 


Mendelian Heredity (continued). The Inheritance 
of Colour. Concurrence of two factors in the pro- 


m CONTENTS 


CHAP. 
duction of Colour.—Colour in Animals; some colours 
‘epistatic’ over others.—Flower-colours ; Reversion on 
crossing.—The nature of albinism.—More complex cases 
of interrelation ; Stocks, Primulas : : ; 71 


VII. Some Disputed Questions. Mendelian segregation 
Inheritance of acquired characters.—Indirect and ex- 
perimental evidence. — Telegony.— Maternal Impres- 
sion ae ; : s : ; 85 


VIII. Heredity in Man. Physical and Mental Characters.— 
Diseases—Mendelian Characters; Eye-colour, Brachy- 
dactyly, abnormalities of the Eye. —Non-Mendelian 
characters; Skin and Hair-colour.— Importance of 
Heredity in relation to Sociology ; ; 99 


ApprENDIx I. Historical Summary of Theories of Heredity. 
Lamarck.—Darwin and the Theory of Pangenesis.— 
Weismann’s Theory of Germ-Plasm_. : EG 


AppENDIX IJ. The Material Basis of Inheritance. The 
Nucleus and Chromosomes as possible ‘bearers’ of 
Heredity. Behaviour of Chromosomes in Germ-cell 


formation : : : : : : : 2 126 
LITERATURE List ; : ‘ : ; : : : 132 
GLOSSARY  . 2 : z 2 ; : : IGS 


INDEX g 5 : : ! ; ; : . ales 


CHAPTER I 
INTRODUCTION 


Durine the whole history of scientific enquiry, 
one of the most fascinating and at the same time one 
of the most baffling of the problems which confront 
mankind has been the cause of the resemblances and 
differences between parents and children. In general, 
the facts are common knowledge; the essence of 
Heredity and Variation is expressed in the proverbs 
‘Like begets like ’ and ‘ Nature never uses the same 
mould twice. Yet clearly the two proverbs are con- 
tradictory, for if like really begets like Nature must 
use the same mould for all the members of a family. 
Our object therefore is to investigate, first, how 
the characters of a parent actually are distributed 
among the children, and how the offspring of the same 
parentage may differ among themselves; and secondly, 
if possible, what is the mechanism by which the re- 
semblances and diversities are brought about. 

These problems are interesting from various points 
of view. They attract us for their own sake, as does 

D. 1 


2 HEREDITY [ CH. 


anything mysterious or unexplained; they have a 
deep human and practical importance, for not only 
do they affect us all individually, but upon their 
solution depends, to an extent as yet only dimly 
realised, the answer to some of our most pressing 
social questions; and finally they lie at the very root 
of all theories of organic evolution, so that they form 
as it were the basis of philosophical biology. The 
relation of the study of Heredity and Variation to 
sociology must be left to a later chapter, but before 
proceeding further we must shortly consider its bearing 
on theories of evolution. 

The fact of organic evolution is admitted by all 
schools of biology, but about the causes of the pro- 
cess and the manner in which it takes place there is 
still wide diversity of opinion. To some of the more 
important theories of evolution it will be necessary 
to refer again later, but however great may be the 
difference of opinion with regard to them, all biologists 
agree that evolution depends ultimately on Variation 
and Heredity. Darwin called his great book The 
Origin of Species because the unit step, so to speak, 
on the scale of evolution is the transition from one 
species to another. But if a species A is to give rise 
to aspecies B, in the first place some individuals of A 
must vary in the direction of B, and then the variation 
must be inherited, for otherwise no permanent change 
can take place. The differences with regard to the cause 


1] HEREDITY 3 


and method of evolution arise therefore partly from 
our ignorance of the laws of variation and heredity, 
and partly from different ideas as to the causes which 
lead to progression in certain directions rather than 
in others. This latter source of disagreement is to a 
large extent outside the province of this book, but 
the subjects of Heredity and Variation are so inti- 
mately bound together that one cannot be adequately 
treated without the other. If, however, we can come 
to any definite decision with regard to the nature of 
_ Heredity and Variation, we shall have made a long 
step towards understanding the method by which 
evolution has taken and is taking place. 


One other point must be mentioned. The study 
of heredity brings us face to face with perhaps the 
most fundamental problem of biology—the ultimate 
nature of living matter. For if an ovum, barely 
visible to the eye, or the much smaller spermatozoon 
which is visible only with high magnification, can 
bear potentially all the parental characters which 
may be inherited by the offspring, it is clear that 
any hypothesis of the nature of living matter must 
take these things into account; and though we 
cannot unravel or even imagine it, we can at least 
get some idea of the amazing complexity of the 
substances which in thoughtless moments we group 
together under the single name of ‘protoplasm.’ 

1—2 


i HEREDITY [ CH. 


We will now attempt, by means of a few examples, 
to illustrate some of the questions which must be 
answered, and some of the facts which must be 
brought into relation, by any consistent account 
of the process of heredity. A tall man on the 
average has taller children than a short man, but 
if all the sons of a number of tali men were measured, 
it would be found that they showed every gradation 
in height between the tallest and shortest; some 
would be taller than the fathers, others shorter, but 
every gradation between them would occur. Also, 
if a tall man marries a short wife, the sons are neither 
all as tall as the father, nor divided sharply into a tall 
group and a short group; again they make a graded 
series from short to tall. But if we cross a tall 
variety of the sweet-pea with a dwarf variety, all 
the offspring are as tall as the tall parent, and among 
the offspring of these crossed talls, some are tall and 
some short, but none are intermediate. Here then 
we get two distinct modes of inheritance, and also 
two kinds of variation; in the first case the character 
varies in such a way that all intermediates are found 
between the extreme conditions, and in the second 
the individuals can be classified sharply into two 
groups. Again, we cross a white mouse or rabbit 
with a black one, and all the offspring may have the 
grey-brown colour of the wild animal—we have pro- 
duced what is called reversion to the wild type, and 


1] HEREDITY 5 


have obtained a form different from either parent. 
But if we mate the same black parent with another 
white individual, it may happen that all the offspring 
are black, and instead of reverting to the wild form 
they all follow one parent. If either the greys or 
blacks produced in this way are mated together, 
some of their young will be white; although none 
of the children of the original white individual 
resembled their white parent in colour, yet the white 
has appeared again among the grandchildren after 
skipping a generation. In man, a colour-blind father 
rarely has colour-blind children, but some of his 
nephews and male grandchildren through the female 
line are usually affected ; that is to say, the disease 
appears in males but is transmitted by females. 

It is clear from this short list of examples that 
there are a number of different forms of hereditary 
transmission, and our object must be, first to classify 
them into groups in which the behaviour is similar, 
and next to attempt to bring them under a common 
scheme. And it is also clear that the different kinds 
of heredity are associated with different kinds of 
variation ; for example variation in height in man is 
inherited differently from variation in colour-vision, 
and both differ from variation of coat-colour in 
rabbits, in their inheritance. 

A question of a different kind is the cause of 
inherited differences, and whether differences due to 


6 HEREDITY [CHI 


the action of circumstances are inherited. Does a 
man, for instance, who develops certain muscles’ by 
frequent use, or who injures his health by excessive 
drinking, have children with larger muscles or poorer 
health in consequence? The question is frequently 
answered in the affirmative, but it is part of the 
province of the study of heredity to investigate the 
matter, and in these and all other cases to decide 
not only whether a character is inherited, but, if it is, 
to what extent and in what manner it will appear in 
the offspring. 


CHAPTER II 
VARIATION 


WE have seen that the subjects of Heredity and 
- Variation are so closely connected that one cannot 
be considered apart from the other, for without 
variation all the offspring of the same parents would 
be exactly alike, and the study of heredity would 
resolve itself into an investigation of the cause of 
this likeness. But the actual problem is much less 
simple ; it includes the questions how and why the 
members of a family may differ from one another, 
and according to what rules and by what means 
these differences are transmitted to later generations. 
In practice therefore the study of heredity is the 
study of the manner and cause of the inheritance of 
variations, and hence the nature of variation must 
be examined before enquiry into its transmission. 

Before the time of Darwin variations were fre- 
quently regarded as abnormalities, inconvenient to 
the systematist and of relatively small importance. 
Every species was supposed to conform to the type 


8 HEREDITY [cH. 


originally created, and divergences from this type 
were regarded as imperfections. But it was obvious 
that there was always more or less fluctuation about 
the type in different individuals, and breeders of 
plants and animals made use of this want of uni- 
formity to select the best specimens and so to 
improve the race. The Natural Selection theory 
of Darwin and Wallace supposes that a process 
comparable with this takes place in nature, and so 
brings about the adaptations of natural species. 

Of the causes which induce variation nothing 
definite was known, but Darwin’s belief was generally 
accepted that it is due to changes in environment 
acting directly or indirectly on the organism. He 
regarded the action of such changes as cumulative 
through a number of generations, so that its effect 
in producing variation might not be visible until the 
change had acted on several generations. This belief 
was founded on the observation that animals bred 
in captivity appear to be much more variable than in 
the wild condition, and the changed conditions of 
life are supposed to induce the variation. But species 
in nature are not by any means subject to uniform 
environment, and thus their variability was ascribed 
to similar causes. 

Darwin and Wallace pointed out that variation 
occurs in all parts of every species, that it appears 
to occur in every possible direction, and to every 


Ir | HEREDITY 9 


extent from very small to considerable range. ‘They 
therefore founded their theory on this type of 
variability rather than on the occurrence of con- 
siderable ‘occasional variations’ which are not 
connected with the type by a series of intermediates. 
It was not, however, until after the theory of 
Natural Selection had obtained general recognition, 
that any detailed study was undertaken of the actual 
frequency and extent of variation, and its mode of 
occurrence. 

The accurate investigation of variation has thus 
been in progress only for some twenty or twenty-five 
years, and according to the methods adopted students 
have become divided into two somewhat distinct 
schools. One of these has devoted itself rather to 
the attempt to observe and classify the different 
kinds of variation, and the other, generally called 
the ‘biometrician’ school, to measure its frequency 
and range. It will be convenient to consider the 
results obtained by the second method first. 

If a character is chosen which can be accurately 
measured, such as human stature, and a sufficiently 
large number of individuals are observed, it will 
commonly be found that there is considerable range 
of variation, and that every gradation in size occurs 
between the smallest and largest. Such variation is 
spoken of as ‘continuous, as opposed to ‘discontinu- 
ous’ variation in which individuals of two kinds occur, 


10 HEREDITY (CH. 


which are not connected by intermediates. Further, 
in cases of continuous variation it will appear that 
one size is more common than any other, and, in the 
simplest cases, that the individuals are progressively 
rarer as the size of the structure considered diverges 
more and more from the most frequent value. The 
most frequent condition is named the ‘mode, and its 
size the ‘modal value’ for the character. For ex- 
ample, if the heights of a large number of men were 
measured, it might be found that they ranged by 
every gradation from 60 to 76 inches. If the measure- 
ments were taken to the nearest inch, it might then 
be found that a greater number had a stature of 
68 inches than any other height, that the next most 
frequent heights were 67 and-69 inches, and that the 
more the stature differed from 68 inches in either 
direction, the fewer would be the men having that 
measurement. This could be represented graphically 
by arranging vertical lines representing the heights 
of every man in order of their height ; a line joming 
their tops would then rise rapidly at the lower end, 
would be nearly flat as it passed over the men having 
heights near the ‘modal value’ of 68 inches, and 
would rise again steeply to the exceptionally tall 
men at the upper end of the row. [13]? 

A more instructive method of graphically repre- 


1 For references see the end of the Volume. 


1] HEREDITY ol 


senting the distribution of variation is to take a base- 
line and divide it into equal parts, each representing 
an equal increment in the structure measured. From 
each division of the base-line a vertical line is drawn 
representing by its length the number of individuals 
having that measurement. 


ie) 100 200 300 400 500 coo 700 800 SOO 1000 


Fig. 1. Curve illustrating stature, the vertical scale representing 
heights above 60 inches, the horizontal scale numbers of indi- 
viduals (up to 1000). 


In the imaginary case taken above, the base-line 
would have 17 divisions representing successive 
heights of from 60 to 76 inches; at each division 
a vertical line is drawn which by its length repre- 
sents the percentage of the population which have 
that height (fig. 2). By joining the tops of the per- 
pendiculars (ordinates) a curve, or more strictly a 
polygon, is obtained which graphically represents the 


12 HEREDITY (CH. 


distribution of the variation among the population 
measured. The highest point of the curve represents 
the mode for the character, and the extremes of 
variation are where it touches the base-line. The 
more numerous are the subdivisions into which the 


ft —_— 


; 


a 
§ 130 + | 


‘ 3 
inal 
D 


& | | | Ly 
+ 100 j ita 
: 

| eee 


Frequency p 


AL BREN 
AR P* Plexi | | | Pee 
52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 7778 79 

Stature in Inches 


Fig. 2. Curves showing distribution of stature in women (mothers) 
—dotted line; andin men (fathers)—continuous line. The curves 
approach the ‘ normal curve.’ (Data from Pearson.) 


variable character is classified, the more nearly the 
line joining the tops of the ordinates will approxi- 
mate to a smooth curve ; e.g. if the population were 
measured to the nearest quarter of an inch instead 
of to the nearest inch there would be four times 


II] HEREDITY 13 


as many ordinates and the curve would be nearly 
smooth. 

It is clear that a curve of this kind can be used 
for comparing the variability of different characters, 
for the greater the variability of the population the 
wider will be the base; consequently the curve for 
a very variable character will be relatively low and 
wide, that for a slightly variable one measured in the 
same scale will be tall and steep. If the curve is 
quite similar on either side of the longest perpen- 
dicular (‘median, representing the modal value), it is 
called a ‘normal curve, and such a curve may be 
obtained by plotting any measurements which vary 
fortuitously around a most frequent value. For 
example, if a large number of beans including equal 
numbers of white ones and black ones were placed in 
a sack, and drawn out ten at a time without selection 
of colour, most frequently five white and five black 
would be drawn, less often six of one colour and four 
of the other, more rarely seven and three and so on 
to the rarest case of ten of one colour. If the numbers 
of white beans in a draw are plotted along the base- 
line, and the ordinates represent the number of draws 
for each combination, a polygon approaching the 
normal curve will be obtained. Variation which 
gives a normal curve when plotted in this way is 
spoken of as normal variation. 

As mentioned above, the steepness of the curve is 


14 HEREDITY [CH. 


a measure of variability, and this can be expressed 
by taking a point in the curve, the perpendicular 
from which to the base-line divides the area, enclosed 
by the curve, the median and the base-line, into two 
equal parts. Or, differently expressed, the perpen- 
dicular divides the curve in such a way that the 
number of individuals between it and the mean is 
the same as that between it and the extreme. The 
distance of this perpendicular may be used as a 
measure of the variability of the character considered, 
for clearly the greater variability (and thus the flatter 
the curve), the further this perpendicular will be 
from the median’. 

In most variable characters, the frequency of 
variation below the mode is not exactly equal to 
that above it, in which case the curve will be steeper 
on one side of the mode than on the other, and the 
average value for the character (‘mean’) will not be 
identical with the mode. For example, if the varia- 
tion in the number of children in a family were 
plotted in this way, the sizes of families would range 
from 0 to about 20, but the most frequent number 
would perhaps be four. Four would then be the 


1 In practice, not this perpendicular, but another rather further 
from the median is used, which for practical purposes is more con- 
venient. The distance of this perpendicular, measured in units of 
the horizontal scale, is called the ‘standard deviation’ and is regu- 
larly employed as a measure of variability. 


11] HEREDITY 15 


modal value, but the average or mean might be about 
six ; the curve would rise steeply to the mode, and 
fall away more gradually to the maximum number 
(fig. 3). Such a curve is described as ‘skew.’ In 


400 
: UE 

oO 

= 
3 300 ager SSS 
o 

2 

: : 


100 ; 1E NI + |_| 
OM mM oWAmS eGe7, bo aE Se 
Size of Family 


Fig. 3. Curve showing distribution of size of 3837 families in 
America containing deaf-mutes. (After Schuster, ‘ Hereditary 
Deafness.’ Biometrika, Vol. 1v. 1906, p. 474.) 


extreme cases the mode is at one end of the curve, 
when variation takes place only on one side of it, 
e.g. in the marsh-marigold, the most frequent number 
of ‘petals’ is five, but there may rarely be six, seven or 
eight, but practically never less than five, so that in 
plotting the frequency a ‘half-curve’ is obtained 
(fig. 4). 


16 HEREDITY (CH. 


Another rather frequent condition is that the 
curve has two maxima or modes (fig. 5), indicating 
that a large number of individuals have a low measure- 
ment, a less number are intermediate, and again a 
larger number have a higher measurement. Species 
which vary in this way are called ‘dimorphic,’ or if 


300 
200 
100 
Slr Gn STS 
Fig. 4. ‘Half-curve’ representing the number of ‘ petals’ on 416 


flowers of the marsh-marigold (Caltha palustris). (After de Vries.) 


there are more than two peaks to the curve, ‘poly- 
morphic.’ Further, it is possible that the two parts 
of the curve should be entirely separate, if inter- 
mediates between the low and high groups are 
completely wanting. This type of variation is spoken 
of as ‘discontinuous’ in contrast to the ‘continuous ’ 


1] HEREDITY 17 


variation hitherto considered. It is possible that 
dimorphic cases in which intermediates exist are 
really essentially discontinuous, but that the two 
groups into which the species is divided each exhibit 
continuous variation about the mode for the group, 
to such an extent that the higher members of one 


Numbers of Individuals 


to 


3 4 5 6 7 8 9 
Lengths of Forceps in Millimetres 


Fig. 5. Curve with two modes, representing frequency of lengths 
of forceps of male EHarwigs from the Farne Islands. (After 
Bateson.) 


group overlap the lower of the other. For example, 

if the modal (most frequent) stature for a race of 

men were 68 inches, and for the women 62 inches, it 

might happen that on plotting a frequency curve for 

the stature of adults including both sexes, a curve 
D. 2 


18 HEREDITY [CH. 


would be obtained having two maxima, one near 62 
and another near 68. Yet the stature might be a 
definite sexual character, and hence essentially as 
discontinuous as the sexes themselves. This distinc- 
tion between continuous and discontinuous variation 
may seem unimportant in itself, but when its in- 
heritance is considered the distinction becomes of the 
first importance. 

Clearer examples of discontinuous variation are 
given by such characters as colour, or by organs 
which are repeated in series, such as vertebrae and 
ribs, the segments of a worm or the petals of a 
flower. When variation occurs in this latter group 
it is generally complete, so that the different forms 
are visibly discontinuous. In the case of colour in 
the skin or hair in animals, or petals of flowers, 
discontinuity is sometimes less apparent, and grading 
frequently occurs, but even in apparently graded 
cases the inheritance of the character may often 
reveal discontinuity. For example, a piebald animal 
might be thought to be intermediate between the 
fully-coloured and albino (white with no pigment), 
but breeding tests would at once show that piebald- 
ness was an independent character, which cannot be 
regarded as in any sense intermediate between the 
other two conditions except in general appearance. 
The same applies to such cases as the ‘silver’ cat or 
rabbit, or the pale purple sweet-pea; the cause of 


It] HEREDITY 19 


the pale colour is entirely distinct from the cause 
of the absence of pigment in the white varieties of 
those species. 

The recognition of the importance of discontinuity 
in variation, which we owe chiefly to the work of 
Bateson in England and De Vries in Holland, is one 
of the chief advances which the study of the subject 
has made since the time of Darwin. 

One other distinction between different kinds of 
variation must be mentioned here, which will be 
discussed more fully in subsequent chapters. The 
kinds of variation mentioned above are all anxborn, or 
inherent in the individual and to a great extent iIn- 
dependent of its manner of life. But it is well known 
that the continued use of an organ or structure, or the 
prolonged action upon it of some external stimulus, 
may alter its form or cause it to assume a condition 
different from that which it would have had if these 
influences had not acted. In general, an organ tends 
to adapt itself either to the uses to which it is put or 
to the action of the environment which surrounds it. 
The muscles of a limb used for strenuous work in- 
crease in size and strength, or a part of the skin 
continually exposed to bright light develops a deeper 
colour than if it is covered. The converse process is 
also true ; an organ which is not used or exposed to 
its normal stimuli tends to diminish, and become less 

99 


I _ 


20 HEREDITY [CH. 


adapted to the use to which it is normally put. Such 
characters as these, arising in response to a stimulus, 
and not appearing in its absence, are technically 
called ‘acquired characters, a phrase which it will be 
necessary to use rather frequently in the following 
pages. As a rule, such ‘acquired characters’ are 
adaptive, that is, they render the organism or 
structure better fitted to its surroundings than if 
they had not been developed. The older students of 
heredity never doubted that these acquired characters 
were inherited as strongly as the inborn characters 
discussed above, but since the publication of 
Weismann’s theory of heredity (see Appendix I) with 
the great body of evidence which he has collected 
on the other side, opinion has turned increasingly 
towards the belief that acquired variations are not 
transmitted. Weismann regards the germ-cells as 
essentially distinct from the rest of the body, so 
that acquired modifications of the body cannot be 
transmitted because the germ-cells are not affected. 
The germ-cells collectively, or rather that part of 
them which is concerned with the transmission of 
hereditary characters, he calls ‘germ-plasm,’ the rest 
of the body consisting of ‘body-plasm,’ and he regards | 
‘acquired’ modifications as affecting body-plasm only. 
A developing germ-cell gives origin to both germ- 
plasm and body-plasm of a new individual, and hence 


11] HEREDITY 2] 


characters borne by the germ-plasm appear in the 
body ; but since body-plasm cannot be converted into 
germ-plasm, modifications of the body cannot be 
transmitted to offspring. 

The possible inheritance of acquired characters is 
treated more fully in a later chapter. 


CHAPTER Ill 
THE CAUSES OF VARIATION 


In the last chapter the distinction has been 
explained between continuous and discontinuous 
variation ; some confusion has however arisen with 
regard to the terms used in describing these conditions. 
Continuous variation about a mean (or more accur- 
ately modal) condition is sometimes spoken of as 
‘fluctuation, but as will be seen below this kind of 
variability probably includes two very distinct groups 
of facts. It may include inherent variability arising 
in the germ-cells, or it may include differences in the 
adult condition having their origin in different effects 
of environment during growth. Some writers have 
used the word ‘fluctuation’ for this latter condition 
only. 

Discontinuous variation is sometimes called 
‘mutation, a word which also has been used in 
several senses. It may mean the appearance of a 
form varying discontinuously from the type, or it 
may be applied to the discontinuous character itself. 


CH. III| HEREDITY 23 


A more serious source of confusion is that the term is 
used by some to denote any discontinuous variation 
arising ‘spontaneously, by others for cases in which 
the variety differs from the type in several apparently 
distinct characters, and not only in one, so that the 
new form constitutes an ‘elementary species.’ Since 
in studying heredity it 1s usually important to con- 
sider distinct characters separately, it may be per- 
missible to use the word for the origin of a form 
differing recognisably from the type and not connected 
with it by true intermediates. 

It has already been pointed out that very little is 
accurately known about the causes of variation, and 
it is not impossible that the different forms of varia- 
tion have different origins. Most writers agree that 
the ultimate cause must lie in the action of environ- 
ment in some form, but as Darwin clearly stated in 
the Origin of Species the environment may act 
directly or indirectly. In variation of size for ex- 
ample, it is clear that the supply of nourishment, 
etc., during growth may have considerable influence 
on the size of the adult, and such variation will 
commonly be continuous owing to the evenly graded 
action on different individuals. In these cases the 
action is direct. If, however, Weismann’s theory of 
germ-plasm and body-plasm is correct, such action may 
affect only the body and not be transmitted to off 
spring. It is also possible that the germ-cells may 


24 HEREDITY [CH. 


be indirectly affected, giving rise to variation in the 
offspring; in such a case, however, there is no 
necessity that the effect on the offspring should be in 
any way similar to the direct effect of the conditions 
on the parent. Nothing is known of the nature of 
possible effects of environment on the germ-cells ; 
the action may possibly be effective immediately and 
give rise to variability in the next generation, or it 
may be that the effects are cumulative and only cause 
visible changes after several generations have been 
exposed to the same influences. Galton [13] suggested 
that the organism may have a certain ‘stability,’ but 
that influences acting for several generations may 
have a cumulative effect which will gradually alter the 
equilibrium until it is finally upset and falls into a 
new condition of stability, giving rise to an apparently 
sudden variation. A chemical analogy may make this 
clearer. If litmus is added to an alkaline solution its 
colour will be blue. Acid may now be added drop 
by drop to neutralize the alkali, and suddenly, when 
the solution becomes acid, the litmus turns red. 
Examples of variation of which this may possibly be 
an analogy will be given below. 

With regard to the action of environment on the 
body many facts are known, but it is not certain that 
they really have any bearing on the question of the 
origin of variation. For variations so produced are 
‘acquired characters, and in many cases at least 


111 | HEREDITY 25 


there is no evidence that they are inherited. For 
example, many butterflies have two generations in 
the year, one of which lives through its whole life- 
history in the summer and the other passes the winter 
as a pupa (chrysalis). In some cases the two genera- 
tions are strikingly different, and it has been shown 
that by freezing the pupae of the summer brood at 
the right stage, specimens like the spring brood can 
be obtained. The difference between the two genera- 
tions is thus due to the action of cold on the pupa. 
But the two forms regularly alternate in nature and 
the effects of cold are not inherited. In plants, some 
species produce quite different leaves according to 
whether they are grown in water or in dry soil, but 
the conditions act on the individual, and do not affect 
its progeny. In such a case, what is inherited is 
the faculty of making a certain definite response to 
definite conditions, and this faculty is present whether 
the conditions operate or not. In man such diseases 
as tuberculosis are commonly called hereditary; this 
however does not mean that the child has the disease 
because his parent had it, but that the parent had 
a constitution liable to that disease, and the child 
inherits a similar constitutional liability. If the 
parent had never been exposed to infection the 
child would still inherit the lability, for what is 
transmitted is not the disease or its effects, but the 
faculty of acquiring it if exposed. It will be found 


26 HEREDITY [CH. 


that most cases which at first sight seem to support 
the theory of the inheritance of acquired characters 
are equally explicable in the view that both parent 
and offspring are susceptible to the action of the ex- 
ternal factor ; what is inherited is not the character 
acquired, but the innate power of acquiring it. 

But it is always possible that some forms of 
external conditions may act on both the body-cells 
and germ-cells concurrently, and produce similar 
effects In each. For example, it may happen that 
extremes of temperature produce striking colour- 
variations in certain butterflies. Weismann has 
pointed out that, according to his theory, in a 
developing butterfly the determinants for producing 
colour not only exist in the germ-cells which will 
transmit the character to the offspring, but also in 
the embryonic cells of the body which go to produce 
the coloured parts of the perfect insect. If extremes 
of heat or cold cause changes in the colour-deter- 
minants in the developing wings, so that abnormal 
colours result, it is possible that the determinants 
in the germ-cells which transmit the colour-pattern 
to the next generation will be similarly modified, so 
that the offspring will show similar abnormalities. 
This would not be the transmission of an ‘acquired 
character’ in the strict sense of the expression, but 
the simultaneous modification of body and germ-cells 
in the same manner. 


111] HEREDITY 27 


But as mentioned above, it is possible that the 
same factor acting on body and germ-cells may pro- 
duce different results in the two cases, so that the 
individual on which the influences have acted may 
show one modification and its offspring another. It 
is also possible that not all the germ-cells will be 
affected alike, and so among the progeny some will 
show modification and others not, or some may be 
differently affected from others ; for the conditions of 
stability of different germ-cells may conceivably be 
different. Certain experiments on insects give reason 
for supposing that this is so. The results obtained 
by Standfuss and others from exposing pupae of 
butterflies and moths to abnormal temperatures, 
while not entirely concordant among themselves, on 
the whole indicate that moderate degrees of heat and 
cold tend to alter in the same way the whole batch of 
insects treated, often in the direction of varieties of 
the species naturally occurring in warmer or colder 
climates. But excessive heat or cold causes extreme 
variations among only a small proportion of the 
insects treated, and among the offspring of these ab- 
normal specimens only a small fraction are abnormal, 
and some of these have not the same abnormality 
as the parents. These observations suggest that 
extreme conditions may upset the stability of the 
type, causing abnormalities to appear, and _ that 
some of the germ-cells may also be altered, but 


28 HEREDITY [CH. 


not necessarily or even usually in the same manner 
as the body-cells. 

An American zoologist, Tower, describes the pro- 
duction of mutations by the action of environment in 
a beetle (Leptinotarsa). In nature he found about 
one such variation among 6000 specimens ; when bred 
in captivity they were more frequent, but when the 
full-grown beetles were exposed to extremes of heat, 
humidity, etc., during the maturation of the eggs, the 
offspring may include a large proportion (over 80 per 
cent.) of ‘mutations.’ These were of several distinct 
kinds, like those rarely found in nature, and when 
bred together they are stated to breed true. In this 
case the abnormal conditions produced no effect on 
the individuals exposed to them, for they already had 
their final form, but as their eggs were matured 
under these conditions the action took effect on the 
eggs, and mutation resulted among the offspring. 
When part of the eggs of an individual were matured 
under abnormal, another part under normal con- 
ditions, mutation occurred only among the offspring 
in the first case, all the beetles in the second being 
normal. It should be noted that as in the experi- 
ments with butterflies the effect of changed conditions 
was not specific ; the same conditions may produce 
more than one kind of mutation in the same batch of 
eggs, and some eggs were not affected at all. In 
both cases the abnormal environment seems to upset 


UT] HEREDITY 29 


the equilibrium, but the effects may differ in different 
individuals. It is the nature of the organism or 
germ-cell affected which determines whether and 
to what extent the change shall take place; the 
environment merely supplies the stimulus. 

It will be seen that our knowledge of the causes 
of variation, in so far as these are connected with 
environment, is very incomplete and unsatisfactory, 
for although it is fairly clear that conditions may 
sometimes disturb the equilibrium of the germ-cells 
and provide a stimulus to variation, yet we have no 
knowledge of the way in which the stimulus acts 
and can make no prediction as to the direction the 
variation will take. Before leaving the subject, one 
other cause of variability must be mentioned—the 
effect of crossing different races in producing varia- 
tion. It frequently happens that the result of 
crossing distinct races is that the crossed individuals 
differ from either parent ; sometimes in the direction 
of increased vigour, as was pointed out by Darwin, 
and other more recent observers ; sometimes by the 
development of characters apparently not possessed 
by either parent, as in the case of ‘reversion on 
crossing. The cause of this latter phenomenon will 
be discussed in a later chapter. In the subsequent 
generations from the cross great diversity may often 
appear, and Darwin supposed that the mingling of 
two distinct germinal stocks had an effect in dis- 


30 HEREDITY [cH 


turbing the equilibrium similar to that produced by 
change of environment. To some extent this is 
doubtless true, but recent developments of the 
theory of heredity have afforded a more exact ex- 
planation, in the recombination of the different 
characters of the two races which are crossed. A 
fuller account of ‘variation induced by crossing’ 
must therefore be postponed until the principles of 
heredity have been discussed. 

One further question should be mentioned before 
proceeding to the subject of heredity, namely, the 
relative importance of ‘inherent’ and ‘acquired’ 
characters in making up the sum of characters of a 
mature individual. It is often assumed, especially ia 
human cases, that the environment has a prepon- 
derating influence in shaping the individual. In a 
certain sense this is true, for many characters can 
only develop in a suitable environment; muscles 
must be exercised to be properly formed and the 
mind cannot develop its full powers if it is never 
used. But the study of variation leads inevitably 
to the conclusion that the inherent characteristics 
are all-important, and that the effect of environment 
is not much more than to give them opportunity to 
develop. This is perhaps most impressively seen im 
the case of ‘identical twins,’ as has been shown by 
Galton [12]. There is reason to believe that such 
twins are produced by the division of one ovum, and 


It | HEREDITY 31 


even if exposed to different conditions they remain 
through life much more alike than ordinary brothers 
who may be brought up under precisely similar 
surroundings. The same fact is still further em- 
phasised by the study of heredity. 


CHAPTER IV 
THE STATISTICAL STUDY OF HEREDITY 


In studying heredity, either of two methods may 
be adopted. We may either choose a character and 
observe or measure its development in a large number 
of parents and in their children, and so deduce the 
average extent of resemblance between parents and 
children for that character; or we may consider a 
number of individual cases separately, and endeavour 
to discover the manner in which the character appears 
in the children who have parents or ancestors pos- 
sessing it. With regard to the first method Prof. 
Pearson has written ‘We must proceed from inheri- 
tance in the mass to inheritance in narrower and 
narrower classes, rather than attempt to build up 
general rules on the observation of individual in- 
stances. And ‘...the very nature of the distribution 
...seems to indicate that we are dealing with that 
sphere of indefinitely numerous small causes, which 
in so many other instances has shown itself only 
amenable to the calculus of chance, and not to 


CH. Iv| HEREDITY 33 


the analysis of the individual instance’ [25, ‘Math. 
Contrib. HI.’ Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. A, 1896, p. 255). 
The second method on the other hand has been used 
in cases where the causes of variation appear to be 
few and definite, and seeks to isolate these causes. 
The first method is thus clearly adapted especially to 
characters which vary continuously and which can 
be measured ; the second to characters which vary 
discontinuously and can be sharply separated into 
classes. The first method gives on the whole the 
average intensity of inheritance, but little information 
with regard to its probable development in individual 
cases; the second attempts to answer the question 
in what manner the character will be distributed 
among the offspring in any family. 

The founder of the modern statistical, or as it is 
now often called, the biometrical study of heredity was 
Sir Francis Galton, and its leading exponents have 
been Professor Karl Pearson and the late Professor 
Weldon. In this chapter an attempt will be made to 
explain the fundamental principles on which the 
biometric methods rest, and to outline the chief 
results obtained; the methods themselves frequently 
require mathematics of an advanced order, and for 
the study of them the reader is referred to the books 
and papers dealing with the subject mentioned in the 
bibliography. 

It has already been seen that in the case of a 

D. 3 


34 HEREDITY , [CH. 


character which varies continuously about a mean or 
mode, the greater the divergence from the mode in 
either direction, the fewer will be the individuals 
showing that divergence. In the case of human 
stature, if the modal height of a population is 68 
inches, there will be fewer men of 64 or 72 inches 
than of 66 or 70, and still fewer of 63 or 73 inches. 
If now the sons of all the men having a given diver- 
gence were measured, and it were found that they 
averaged as great a divergence from the mode as 
their fathers, it is clear that on the average the 
height of the sons would equal that of their fathers. 
This does not mean that every son would exactly 
resemble his father in stature, but the sons would 
vary about the paternal stature equally above and 
below it, and when plotted in a curve their statures 
would make a curve having the paternal stature as 
its mode. The average stature of the sons would 
then be completely determined by the stature of the 
fathers. If on the other hand the stature of the 
father had no relation with that of his sons, it is 
clear that the statures of the sons of fathers of any 
height would vary about the mean of the general 
population considered. In practice it is found that 
the modal value for sons of fathers of a given height 
is between the height of their fathers and the mode 
of the general population. That is to say, if the fathers 
diverge a given amount from the general mode, their 


Iv] HEREDITY 35 


sons will on the average diverge less ; they will vary 
about a modal value lying between the general mode 
and the fathers’ measurement. This fact is called 
‘regression. It sometimes seems paradoxical to those 
who have not considered it that the mean deviation 
of children from the general mode is always less than 
that of their parents. But of course it does not mean 
that all sons of tall fathers will be shorter than their 
fathers ; some will be as tall or taller, but the sons of 
a number of fathers of given stature will vary about 
a mode lying between the fathers’ stature and the 
mode of the whole population. 

Now it is plain that the amount of regression is a 
measure of the intensity of inheritance ; if the modes 
for sons of fathers of every deviation have deviations 
nearly as great as those of the fathers, the intensity of 
inheritance would be high ; if the modes for the sons 
deviate but slightly from the general mode, whatever 
be the deviation of the fathers, the intensity would 
be low. A definite case may make this clearer. 
Suppose the modal stature of the population is 68 
inches ; it might then be found that for fathers of 64 
inches (ie. deviating 4 inches below), the height of 
the sons ranged from 61 to 72 inches. If, however, 
their modal value had a deviation only slightly less 
than the fathers’ deviation, say with a mode at 65 
inches, the regression would be slight and the intensity 
of inheritance high ; if the sons’ mode had a deviation 

3—2 


36 HEREDITY [CH. 


much less than the fathers’, say at 67 inches, the 
regression on the general mean would be considerable 
and the intensity of inheritance low. If then we can 
find means of determining the ratio between the 
deviation of sons in general and the deviation of their 
parents, we shall have a measure of the intensity of 
inheritance for the character considered. This ratio 
is called the ‘coefficient of correlation’ between 
father and son for that character. It should be 
noticed that correlation simply means that two 
quantities vary in relation to each other; the corre- 
lation between parents and children is a convenient 
method of estimating the intensity of inheritance, 
but correlation exists between any two related 
variables, e.g. between the measurements of two 
limbs in the same individual, such as an arm and a 
leg, or between the numbers obtained in successive 
throws of dice, if not all the dice are picked up for 
the second throw. The correlation between the same 
measurement in brothers may be used as a measure 
of inheritance, for two brothers resemble each other 
more than two chance individuals because ne are 
children of the same parents. 

The principle of obtaining a coefficient of corre- 
lation between father and sons is as follows. It will 
be convenient to assume that the variability of the 
character considered is normal, i.e. that the frequency 
curve falls evenly on either side of the mode, so that 


Iv] HEREDITY 37 


the mode is identical with the mean. Stature in 
Inches may be taken as an example. A large number 
of fathers and a son of each are measured to the 
nearest inch; it can then be found what is the 
average measurement of sons for fathers of each 


each class of Father (marked x) 


Mean Statures of Sons for 


75 IN 
76 NS 


77 
6O 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 757677 
Statures of Fathers 


Fig. 6. Diagram of correlation between fathers and sons. 


height from the lowest value to the highest. It will 
be found that the mean deviation of the sons from 
the mean of the population is less than the deviation 
of the fathers for each class of fathers. The average 
ratio between the mean deviations of the sons to the 


38 HEREDITY [CH. 


deviations of the fathers is then the coefficient of 
correlation between father and son for this character’. 
This is more clearly seen in diagram form. 

If a square is made with its sides divided into 
equal lengths corresponding to equal increments in 
stature from 60 to 76 inches, the top may represent 
the scale of statures of fathers and the side the scale 
of mean statures of sons for each class of fathers. If, 
then, there were complete correlation between fathers 
and sons, the mean stature of sons of fathers 62 inches 
high would be 62, of fathers of 63 inches, 63, of 64, 
64 and so on. If on the other hand there were ho 
correlation, the means of the sons of every class of 
father would be the mean of the population (68). 

In the first case the line joining the points repre- 
senting the means of the sons would be a diagonal 
running from corner to corner (AB), in the second 
case a horizontal line running across the middle (CD). 
But if the correlation is between these extremes the 
line would lie between the diagonal and the horizontal 
(EF), and the greater the correlation the steeper 
would be the slope of EF. The steepness of this line 
is thus a measure of correlation, and since all these 
lines pass through O in the middle of the square, the 


1 Ttis assumed throughout that the variability of the sons is similar 
to that of the fathers. If their variability were different this would 
have to be allowed for. The variation is also assumed to be normal, 
so that the mode in each case coincides with the mean. 


Arye] HEREDITY 39 


slope is measured by the size of the angle EOC. The 
angle made by the diagonal at O is 45°, the tangent 
of which is 1 (unity). If there were no correlation 
the angle would vanish, EF coinciding with CD, and 
the correlation coefficient would be 0. Intermediates 
are represented by the value of the tangent of the 
angle EOC. In practice it rarely happens that the 
points representing the means of the sons for each 
class of fathers lie in a perfectly straight line ; when 
they approach it closely the correjation is called 
‘linear’; when they depart from it considerably, it 
is called ‘skew.’ 


Table of intensity of parental inheritance in different 
species. (From Pearson.) 


Number 
Mean of Pairs 
Species Character value used 
Man Stature 506 4886 
Span "459 4873 
Forearm 418 4866 
Kye Colour 495 4000 
Horse Coat Colour 522 4350 
Basset Hound Coat Colour 524 823 
Greyhound Coat Colour 507 9279 
Aphis Ratio of right Antenna 439 368 
(Hyalopterus to Frontal Breadth 
trirhodus) (non-sexual reproduction) 
Water-flea Ratio of Basal Joint of 466 96 
(Daphnia magna) Antenna to Body length 


(non-sexual reproduction) 


40 HEREDITY [ CH. 


Prof. Pearson and his collaborators have worked 
out the correlation between parent and child for a 
number of measurable characters in Man, Animals, 
and Plants, and they find that the numbers group 
themselves about a value not far from 0°48, varying 
from 0°42 to 0°52. That is to say, on the average the 
offspring deviate from the mean about half as much 
as the parent. 

The parental correlation hitherto discussed has 
taken no account of the second parent, for if in- 
dividuals mate at random the one parent may be 
considered alone, and the second will on the average 
have the mean value for the general population. But 
it is clear that one may take for the parental value 
in each class the mean of the two parents (making 
allowance for any difference in measurement due to 
sex), and plot the means of the sons (or daughters) 
against the classes so produced. The value derived 
from taking the mean of father and mother is called 
the ‘mid-parent, and the correlation so arrived at 
would give the measure of resemblance between 
children and their mid-parents. This is naturally 
higher than the correlation observed when only one 
parent is considered; for if both parents deviate in 
the same direction from the mode of the population, 
the children will average a greater deviation than if 
only one does so, and still more than if one deviates 
in one direction, the other in the opposite. We thus 


Iv] HEREDITY 41 


obtain a measure of the amount contributed to the 
offspring by the two parents together, but even now 
we do not find the correlation complete (1:0) because 
the contributions from previous ancestors have also 
to be taken into account. 

Galton was the first to introduce the idea of the 
‘mid-parent,’ and he went on to attempt to estimate 
the average contribution to the children from each 
generation of ancestors. Since the correlation be- 
tween offspring and mid-parent is not complete, 
part of the heritage, which is not visibly present in 
the parents, must be contributed from more distant 
ancestors. Galton concluded from the data he collected 
that on the average half the heritage of an individual 
may be taken as derived from the two parents, one 
quarter from the four grandparents, one eighth from 
the great grandparents, and so on, the whole series 
(4, +, 4, 4—) adding up to unity. Pearson estimates 
the average correlation between offspring and one 
parent, as about ‘5, of offspring with a grandparent 
as °33, with a great grandparent as ‘22, the correlation 
coefficient with an ancestor of each generation being 
2 of that of the next below; these numbers, however, 
are not in any way comparable with Galton’s series 
"5, 25, 125, etc. Galton attempted to estimate the 
amount of the heritage received from the ‘mid- 
ancestor’ of each generation independently of what 
was received from other generations ; but in the 


4 HEREDITY [CH. 


N) 


metaphor of bequests of property, he calculated that 
of the total heritage of an individual, half on the 
average was bequeathed by the parents, one quarter 
by the grandparents direct to the grandchild and so 
on. Pearson’s series ‘5, ‘33, '22 etc. gives the average 
measure of resemblance between children and an 
ancestor of each generation, which is clearly a totally 
different thing. From this series he has worked out 
figures corresponding to Galton’s, making the series 
‘6244, (1988, °0630, i.e. he finds that the parental 
bequest is greater and the ancestral bequests less 
than Galton estimated. From the results obtained 
first by Galton and later by Pearson has been formu- 
lated the ‘Law of Ancestral Heredity, which has 
been stated in various forms, perhaps the most 
general being ‘the mean character of the offspring 
can be calculated with the more exactness, the 
more extensive our knowledge of the corresponding 
characters of the Ancestry’ (Yule[44]). But it should 
be noted that there is an important difference between 
Galton’s original statement of the law, and the later 
statements of Prof. Pearson. Galton wrote that ‘the 
two parents between them contribute on the average 
one-half of each inherited faculty, each of them 
contributing one-quarter. The four grandparents 
contribute between them one-quarter, or each of 
them one-sixteenth; and so on. He regarded this 
as a physiological statement of the way faculties 


Iv | HEREDITY 43 


were transmitted, while Pearson, in his later writings 
at least, regards the law simply as a statistical 
description of what is found when large numbers 
are observed in mass. 

It has been mentioned that the characters which 
especially lend themselves to statistical treatment 
are those which vary continuously and which can be 
accurately measured, but Prof. Pearson has applied 
similar methods to discontinuous characters, which 
can be classified into groups but not measured, for 
example coat-colour in horses. He finds as the 

results of his enquiries that the inheritance of such 
characters can be stated in terms similar to those 
obtained with measurable characters, so that the 
principle of ancestral correlation leading up to the 
law of ancestral heredity may be applied to these 
characters also. But whatever may be the case with 
characters which vary continuously, it will be seen 
below that discontinuous characters are commonly 
alternative in their inheritance, i.e. there is no 
blending, but the offspring exhibit one or other only ; 
and in some at least of these cases, the character of 
the offspring cannot be calculated with any more 
exactness if the ancestry is known than if it is not. 
Such instances show clearly that although the law 
may be statistically true when applied to considerable 
populations, it gives us no clue to the physiological 


44 HEREDITY [cH. 


processes which determine the transmission of char- 
acters from one generation to another. 

Another argument that has been used against the 
physiological validity of the law of ancestral heredity 
is based on the work of Johannsen and others who 
have obtained results similar to his in other cases. 
Johannsen worked at the inheritance of weight of 
seeds in beans and in barley, and self-fertilised the 
plants investigated for a series of generations so as 
to isolate what he calls ‘pure lines... He found that 
in beans, for example, the seed-weights of a mixed 
population gave a normal frequency curye—the 
weights varied continuously and evenly about a mean 
value. The beans on an individual plant when the 
flowers are self-fertilised also form a normal curve 
about a mean, but this mean is not necessarily identical 
with that of the race in general. If now the flowers 
on such an individual are self-fertilised, and the beans 
produced are sown, the mean weight of the beans on 
all the daughter plants will be identical with the 
mean of the beans on the parent, i.e. among the 
offspring produced by self-fertilisation there is no 
regression towards the mean of the race. It thus 
makes no difference whether large or small seeds are 
chosen within the pure line ; the mean weight of the 
seeds on plants grown from the smallest and largest of 
the parental beans (seeds) is in each case equal to the 


Iv] HEREDITY A5 


parental mean. Selection, therefore, within the pure 
line has no effect in altering the mean weight of the 
seeds, for the differences in seed weight within the 
line are not inherited. The probable cause of this 
is that the differences between the seeds on a self- 
fertilised plant are due to the action of external 
circumstances ; the position of the beans in the 
pod or the position of the pods on the plant cause 
differences in the nutrition which allow some beans 
to grow larger than others. These differences are 
‘acquired characters, and we have here additional 
evidence that such are not inherited. It is to variation 
of this type that the term ‘fluctuation’ is applied by 
some authorities. 

It is clear then that if selection is made among 
beans harvested from a mixed population, on the 
whole the larger beans will belong to pure lines 
having a higher mean, and thus selection for a few 
generations will isolate pure lines having a high 
value, and the mean of successive generations will 
rise until the largest pure lines have been isolated. 
Beyond that point further selection will have no 
effect. This is precisely the result arrived at by 
Prof. Pearson from a study of selection within a 
mixed population ; the mean will rise rapidly on the 
first selection, more slowly later, until in very few 
generations it reaches a point at which selection 
has no appreciable effect. Pearson calculates that if 


46 HEREDITY (CH. 


selection now ceases, the selected race will very 
slowly revert towards the mean of the general popu- 
lation. But, as has been seen, this conclusion is based 
on the assumption that continuous variation is due to 
the concurrent action of an indefinite number of small 
independent causes. If, however, Johannsen is correct, 
we may divide these causes into two classes: the 
causes which induce ‘fluctuation’ as explained above, 
which agree with Pearson’s requirements, and the 
cause or causes which give rise to the difference 
between one pure line and another. Now this second 
group may conceivably consist in a single factor of 
the nature of a small ‘mutation, and if so, by isolating 
the pure line this factor is also isolated, and no return 
towards the mean of the general population need 
take place. According to Johannsen this isolation 
can be effected in one generation by selecting the 
self-fertilised plants which have the highest average 
yield, instead of selecting the heaviest beans them- 
selves. 

We thus obtain by experiments such as those of 
Johannsen a new conception of the possible nature of 
continuous variation ; it may be due partly to ‘ fluctua- 
tion’ brought about by the action of environment 
and not inherited, partly to a series of small step- 
wise ‘mutations, each of which owing to fluctuation 
overlaps the next, and can only be isolated when it is 
possible to breed pure lines. It should be said that 


Iv] HEREDITY 47 


there is as yet no certainty that this account of 
continuous variation is sufficient to cover all cases ; 
it is a suggestion of possibility rather than a state- 
ment of fact. 

We have seen that there is reason to believe that 
the Law of Ancestral Inheritance is true only when 
applied to a large number of individuals considered 
in mass, or, as it has been put, that it is a statistical 
rather than a physiological law. In individual cases 
it is not true that the offspring need be influenced 
by ancestors beyond the parents, but in other cases, 
as will be seen in dealing with Mendelian heredity, 
these ancestors have important effects, so that 
statistically it is possible to say what is the average 
influence of the ancestors of any generation upon 
the offspring. Now in cases where it is possible to 
define rigidly single characters, much more is learned 
from the physiological than from the statistical 
method, but where no such rigid separation of 
characters is possible the statistical law is the only 
one that can be applied. This is particularly the 
case in characters which vary continuously, or where 
the categories into which the character falls overlap 
one another, as for example in Johannsen’s beans. 
Further, the statistical method is frequently the only 
one which is available when experiment is impossible 
and when our knowledge of the facts is based solely 
on numerical data from observed cases, and this of 


48 HEREDITY [CH. 


course applies especially to inheritance in Man, 
where experimental evidence is not available. 

By collecting family histories of distinguished 
men, Galton showed long ago [15] that exceptional 
mental qualities were inherited; and this work 
has recently been much extended and made more 
definite by Professor Pearson and his school. It 
is commonly believed that exceptionally gifted men 
do not have distinguished sons, but this like many 
other popular beliefs is only partly true. It 
has been seen that if an individual deviates a 
certain amount from the general mean, his children 
will on the average deviate less, because when the 
whole ancestry is taken into account, the effect of 
previous generations is to cause regression on the 
mean of the population. And since the theory of 
regression depends on the assumption that variation 
is due to the existence of a large number of inde- 
pendent causes acting concurrently, it is unlikely 
that among the limited number of offspring of one 
exceptional man any one child will unite in himself 
the same combination of factors as went to make up 
the father’s character. Further, it is improbable that 
an unusually gifted man will marry a wife equally 
gifted in the same manner, and the mother’s influence 
on the children is nearly similar to that of the father. 
It cannot therefore be expected that all great men 
should have equally great sons, but they are far more 


Iv] HEREDITY 49 


likely to have exceptional sons than are mediocre 
men, and if the mother is also exceptional in the 
same direction this probability is greatly increased. 

In the last few years the intensity of inheritance 
in such characters has been given numerical ex- 
pression. Professor Pearson, after working out the 
statistical laws of inheritance in many physical char- 
acters of man, animals and plants, has applied the 
same methods to what are called the mental and 
moral attributes. Characters were chosen such as 
vivacity, popularity, conscientiousness, temper, ability, 
-hand-writing, which were estimated by reports from 
school-teachers on the children in their schools ; 
and also intellectual ability as shown in university 
examinations or by the position in a public school 
at a particular age (Schuster [10])'. All these, when 
Investigated by the same methods as were devised 
for the coat-colour of horses or eye-colour in man, 
are found to give results closely in accord with those 
obtained for physical features. The conclusion is 
therefore reached that not only bodily characters, 
but also those of the mind are essentially determined 
by the hereditary endowment received from the 
parents. This result is of great importance practi- 


1 In these characters the resemblance between parent and child 
cannot of course be estimated directly, but it has been pointed out 
above that the resemblance between brothers may be used as a test 
of the intensity of heredity. 


D. 4 


50 HEREDITY [CH 


cally; it shows how little room is left in the 
development of the individual for the effects of 
environment even on the intellect or mind in the 
broadest sense of the word; no doubt the direction 
which intellectual development takes is to a con- 
siderable extent determined by circumstances, but 
the kind of mind is irrevocably decided before the 
child is born. Still less is there room for the 
inheritance of the mental acquirements made by the 
individual during his life, and hence the hopes held 
out of improving the race by education and by 
special care of the dull or feeble-minded are illusory, 
except in so far as they improve the tradition. Just 
as the welfare of the race may be increased by an 
invention which is handed on from generation to 
generation, so the good effects of education or other 
improved conditions may be handed on, but this is 
not heredity. The father may educate his children 
because he himself was educated, but the mental 
powers of his children will be the same whether he 
had a good education or none. And the effects of 
special care given to the weakly or feeble-minded 
may be absolutely harmful to the race, if the im- 
provement so effected leads to more frequent 
marriage among such unfortunates than would other- 
wise be the case, for then an increased number of 
defective children may be born, and the race-average 
be lowered. Hence has arisen the study known as 


Iv] HEREDITY 51 


‘Kugenics, the study, that is, of the methods by 
which the race may be improved both physically and 
mentally. The whole trend of the results obtained 
is that in order to produce exceptionally gifted men 
in both body and mind, those with high development 
of the characters desired should be encouraged to 
marry; and that to prevent the production of the 
weakly and feeble-minded, the only method is to 
prevent such from having offspring. It is admitted that 
at present these things hardly come within ‘ practical 
_ politics, but there is little doubt that the nation 
which first finds a way to make them practical will 
in a very short time be the leader of the world. 


CHAPTER V 
MENDELIAN HEREDITY 


In the last chapter the distinction has more 
than once been referred to between the statistical 
rules of inheritance discovered by observing great 
numbers of cases taken together, and the physio- 
logical laws which determine the actual manner of 
transmission in individual cases. The province of 
the present chapter is to indicate the methods by 
which one at least of these physiological laws has been 
investigated, and the results to which such work 
has led. In studying this part of the subject it is 
necessary to consider, at least in the first place, 
characters which vary and are inherited discontinu- 
ously, so that they may be sharply marked into 
distinct categories. The foundation of the study 
was laid by Johann Gregor Mendel, a monk of the 
monastery of Briinn in Bohemia. His most important 
paper was published in 1866 [2], but perhaps owing 
to the fact that the biological world was then 


CH. v| HEREDITY 53 


occupied almost solely with the discussion of the 
‘Origin of Species, his work attracted no attention 
at the time, and only became celebrated on _ its 
rediscovery in 1900. One cannot avoid speculating 
on the possible effects on biological thought, had 
the experiments and conclusions of his now famous 
contemporary ever come to the knowledge of 
Darwin. 

The method which led Mendel to his great 
discovery was to experiment with plants exhibiting 
discontinuous characters, and to consider each char- 
acter separately. Previous workers in the same 
field had made many laborious experiments in 
crossing different races of plants or animals [7], but 
had always regarded the individual as the unit, and 
hence arose the belief that mongrels or hybrids were 
usually intermediate between the parents, resembling 
one in some features, the other in others, but with 
no regular rule ; and further, that when hybrids were 
bred together the offspring were often almost infinitely 
variable, extending in a series from some closely 
approaching one original parent through a diversity 
of intermediate or new forms to others like the second 
parent. So grew up the belief that the crossing 
of distinct races or breeds is a potent cause of 
variability, which, however, except when ‘reversion 
on crossing’ took place, seemed to fall under no 
ascertainable law. 


54 HEREDITY [CH. V 


Mendel’s most important experiments were made 
with races of the edible pea, which he grew in the 
garden of his monastery. He found in peas several 
characters which vary and are inherited discon- 
tinuously, and he crossed together races which 
differed in one or more of such characters, but in the 
offspring and later generations he considered the 
distribution of each character by itself, quite apart 
from the other characters of the plant. As an 
example we may take the character height or 
tallness. Certain varieties of peas grow stems some 
six feet in height, others are short and do not exceed 
about two feet. The heights fluctuate about a mode, 
but the smallest individuals of one race (grown under 
proper conditions) are taller than the largest of the 
other, and each race breeds true. Similar tall and 
short races exist in the sweet-pea (fig. 7), the short 
race being called ‘Cupid’ sweet-peas. When the 
two races are crossed—and reciprocal crosses give 
identical results—the offspring are not intermediate 
but all are tall, perhaps taller than the tall parents. 
When now. these hybrid talls are self-fertilised, 
among the plants produced some are tall and others 
short, but again none are intermediate. Mendel 
regarded the tallness or shortness as distinct alter- 
native characters, and since tallness alone appears in 
the first cross, he spoke of it as ‘dominant,’ and the 
shortness, which disappeared when crossed with the 


(‘uosoyeq wor) ‘poas Jo pod suo Wory OUIvO SfABMp GOAT] PUB BIT[C4 
eAy og, “(pidng) Javap x vad-yooas []B} SSO1D OTT} WLOTF uoneieueds (*7) puooss oy} ur syuvld 4uystg “LZ Po) As | 


SIIME ¢ 


SJAVMC 


56 HEREDITY [CH. 


dominant tallness, he called ‘recessive. More recent 
work has indicated that a dominant character pos- 
sesses some factor which is absent in its recessive 
alternative; in the present example the stem has 
the power of continued growth which is absent in 
the short pea. Dominance and recessiveness may 
thus be regarded as presence and absence respectively 
of the factor in question ; but since the presence or 
absence of the factor may often give rise to the 
appearance of an alternative pair of characters, 
such a pair have been named by Bateson a pair of 
‘allelomorphs. When a tall pea is crossed with 
a short, the factor tallness is introduced from the 
tall parent, and thus all the offspring are tall. These 
are called the first filial generation, or more shortly 
the generation fF. When these hybrid (Ff) talls are 
self-fertilised, their offspring (second filial or F, 
generation) consist of talls and shorts. Now it has 
been seen that if the factor tallness is present it 
makes itself visible, and therefore the short peas in 
F, should contain no tall factor. And in fact when 
self-fertilised, or fertilised with the original short 
stock, they give only short offspring for as many 
generations as the experiment has been carried to. 
The tall factor has thus apparently been completely 
eliminated from these short peas. 

Further, Mendel found that among the talls in the 
F,, generation, some breed true to tallness when self- 


vy] HEREDITY 57 


fertilised, while others again give a mixture of talls 
and shorts. The whole result may be clearer in 
symbolic form. If 7’ stands for the tall factor, ¢ for 
its absence (shortness), the following results appear. 
(The ‘27%’ in F, will be explained immediately. ) 


Original parents DUS 
} | 
FF, Ih 
Se ee “ 
Fy uly 2Tt tt 
| or 
2 Ie PP aise WING Ele ti 


It is thus clear that among the offspring of the F,, 
(hybrid) generation, some (f¢) have eliminated the 
tall factor altogether and show no difference from 
their short ancestors ; others (77) have nothing but 
the tall factor and thus breed true to tallness ; and 
a third group, which Mendel found was twice as 
numerous as either of the others (therefore marked 
277), proved, by giving mixed offspring when selfed, 
that it is hybrid like its FP’, parent. _ 

The explanation offered by Mendel of these facts 
was as follows. The original tall plant produces 
germ-cells (‘gametes’) bearing tallness ; the short 
plant produces gametes bearing shortness (absence 
of tallness). The F, (hybrid) thus contains both 
conditions ; its cells, resulting from the union of two 
gametes, may be regarded as double structures, con- 


58 HEREDITY [CH. 


taining a double set of determinants for the various 
characters of the plant, one determinant of each 
pair being derived from the male parent, the other 
from the female. An individual produced by union 
of two germ-cells (gametes) and having this double 
character is called a ‘zygote. The F, zygote thus 
contains a determinant for tallness derived from one 
parent, and a corresponding determinant in which 
the tall factor is absent derived from the second 
parent. Now Mendel’s hypothesis to account for the 
observed facts was that although the zygote produced 
by union of tall-bearing and short-bearing gametes 
contains both factors, yet when this hybrid zygote 
gives rise to gametes, it produces some bearing 
tallness and others bearing shortness, but none 
bearing both determinants ; ie. that the alternative 
characters segregate from each other in the forma- 
tion of the gametes, and that gametes bearing one 
or other of the two conditions are formed in equal 
numbers. Since large numbers of gametes of each 
kind are formed, and since they meet indiscriminately 
in fertilisation, a tall will equally often meet a tall or 
a short, and a short will equally often meet a tall and 
a short, and the combinations will thus be in the ratio 
of ATT, 176, VT, Vee, or- PT, 27) Vie ees 
hypothesis is true, it can be tested by fertilising the 
F, hybrid zygote with the pure parental types; the 
F, zygote produces equal numbers of 7’ and ¢ gametes, 


Vv] HEREDITY 59 


the pure short race produces only f, so the offspring 
of the hybrid and the original short should give equal 
numbers of hybrid talls and pure shorts. Similarly 
the hybrid zygote crossed with the pure tall should 
give equal numbers of pure talls (77) and hybrid 
talls (7). Mendel found that this expectation was 
in fact verified by experiment. The whole series 
may be made clearer by a diagram (p. 60), in which 
the zygotes are represented by squares, the gametes 
by circles. 

The middle part of this diagram represents the 
production of the F, zygote and its offspring when 
self-fertilised, producing equal numbers of 7’ and ¢ 
gametes (four of each being represented) and thus 
giving offspring in the ratio of 177, 27%, ltt; the 
sides of the diagram represent the results of crossing 
back the F’, zygote with the parental types 7'7' and ft. 

At this point it is necessary to explain certain 
convenient technical terms introduced by Bateson. 
It has already been mentioned that a pair of alter- 
native characters which segregate in the gametes, as 
described, are called allelomorphs. When an indi- 
vidual is produced by two gametes bearing different 
allelomorphs, so that it contains both members of 
a pair, it is called a ‘heterozygote, or is said to be 
‘heterozygous’ in respect of the character considered, 
e.g., an individual of constitution 7%¢ is heterozygous 
in respect of tallness. If it contains only one kind 


HEREDITY [CH. 


60 


am 


+} 


\ 
\ 


\ 
\ 
\ x 
\ 
N 
XN 
» 


on 


5 
=n 


Les) [4 


NZ 


OO: 


a 


: 


mil 


hi 


dl 


saz0bhiz °4 


1) fo sajauny 


sazobhz *4 


S}2UBIDd 
{0 saJaungy 


82UA1Dd 


v] HEREDITY 61 


of allelomorph of a pair it is a ‘homozygote, e.g. 
individuals of composition 77 and ¢¢ are ‘homozygous’ 
for tallness and shortness respectively. As will be 
seen immediately, it is possible for an individual to 
be heterozygous for one pair of allelomorphs and 
homozygous for another. The essence of Mendel’s 
theory is that owing to the segregation of allelomorphs 
from each other in the production of the gametes of 
a heterozygote, the homozygous offspring, when self- 
fertilised or mated with others of like constitution, 
breed true to the character in question irrespective 
of their ancestry. As far as observation can show, 
the homozygous individuals 7'7' and ¢é in the genera- 
tion F’, breed as true to tallness or shortness as did 
their pure-bred grandparents, in spite of the fact 
that they are the offspring of a cross. 

Hitherto the original parents have been considered 
as differing from each other in only one pair of alter- 
native characters (allelomorphs), but Mendel found 
that in the pea there were several such pairs of 
characters. For example, some races of peas have 
purple flowers, others white; these behave quite 
similarly to tallness and shortness. The purple 
flower contains a factor lacking in the white ; when 
therefore purple is crossed with white, the purple 
colour is dominant and the heterozygote (7, hybrid) is 
purple. Such a heterozygous purple if self-fertilised 
yields 75 per cent. of purple offspring and 25 per cent. 


62 HEREDITY [ CH. 


of white ; the whites and one in every three of the 
purples so produced are ‘extracted’ homozygotes, 
being pure for whiteness or purpleness respectively, 
and therefore breeding true, while the remaining 
purples are heterozygous and when ‘selfed’ will give 
both colours among their offspring. 

If now a tall purple-flowered pea is crossed with 
a short white-flowered, the heterozygous offspring 
will be tall with purple flowers, for both these 
characters are dominant. In the production of 
their gametes (pollen-cells and egg-cells) segregation 
will take place between tallness and shortness, and 
between purpleness and whiteness, but as these pairs 
of characters are totally independent of one another 
they may be associated in any combination as long as 
both members of a pair do not occur in the same 
gamete. Gametes will thus be produced of four 
kinds ; if P represents purple, p its absence (white) ; 
T' tallness and ¢ its absence (shortness), the gametes 
produced by an individual heterozygous in both 
characters will be PT, pT, Pt, pt, with equal numbers 
ofeach. Since these will meet one another at random 
in fertilisation, the F, generation will consist of 
individuals (zygotes) made up of all possible com- 
binations of these four types of gametes, viz. in the 
proportion of 4Pp7t, 2PpTT, 2P-P1T,, Weise 
2Ppti, \PPtt; 2npTt, lppTT ; \pptt. 

Since purple is dominant over white and tall over 


v] HEREDITY 63 


short, the first four types of zygote, which all contain 
both P and 7, will be purple talls; the next two 
containing P but no 7 will be purple short ; the two 
containing 7’ but not P will be tall white, and the 
last with neither P nor 7 will be short white. The 
F’, offspring will thus appear in the ratio of 9 purple 
tall, 3 purple short, 3 white tall, 1 white short. 
Further, of the first group one will be homozygous 
in both characters (PP7T), four homozygous in one 
and heterozygous in the other (2PP7t, 2PpTT) and 
four heterozygous in both (Pp7t). Of the remainder, 
one in each class will be homozygous in both char- 
acters, and the others heterozygous in one, the 
homozygous (pure) types being PPtt, ppTT and 
ppte. 

It is clear then that by crossing two races which 
differ in two allelomorphic characters, and _self- 
fertilising (or mating together) the crossed individuals, 
in the F’, generation a definite proportion of new 
pure combinations are produced. In the above 
example, by crossing tall purple with short white, 
in the second generation not only these types are 
produced, but also short purple and tall white, and 
by selecting the pure (homozygous) individuals pure 
races of these new types are immediately established. 
We thus obtain a new conception of organic char- 
acters, as factors which can be replaced by alternative 
characters without otherwise altering the constitution 


64 HEREDITY [ CH. 


of the organism. The process is comparable with 
a chemical reaction, where one element may replace 
another in a compound ; for example, by mixing silver 
nitrate with sodium chloride, silver chloride and 
sodium nitrate are produced. Or a grosser analogy 
may be taken from bricks in a wall; a red brick may 
be removed and replaced by a blue or a yellow one 
without altering the rest of the wall, and similarly in 
pea-plants by the process described white flowers 
may be replaced by purple, or yellow seed by green. 
After the fact of the segregation of allelomorphic 
characters in the production of the germ-cells of 
a heterozygote, the most striking result of Mendelian 
investigation is this discovery of the independence of 
characters belonging to different pairs. 

That these results are not of merely academic 
interest is shown by the work of Prof. Biffen on 
wheat. Some valuable wheats are liable to the 
attacks of a ftingus giving rise to the disease called 
‘rust, other less valuable races are immune.  Biffen 
has found that by crossing the two races together, 
fertilising the hybrids (#,) among themselves, and 
selecting the homozygous plants in the F’, generation, 
wheat can be produced which combines the valuable 
features of one race with the immunity to rust of the 
other, and so a new and most useful variety of wheat 
is produced. This is only one out of many examples 
that could be given of the possibility of combining 


v] HEREDITY 65 


into one race of wheat the characters previously found 
in different varieties. 

The chief reason that breeders of plants and 
animals believe that the race is permanently con- 
taminated by crossing different breeds is that 
commonly two breeds differ in several or many 
pairs of characters. If two pairs of allelomorphic 


a 


A aut ie a Dy en ns 


x r 
Mac = et 2S bE I; hig {.. Ԥ A 


Fig. 8. A cob of Maize borne by an F, plant from the cross smooth 
(starchy) seed x wrinkled (sugary) seed, fertilised with its own 
pollen, showing about three smooth (dominant) to one wrinkled 
(recessive) seeds on the same cob. (From Bateson.) 


characters are combined in the heterozygote, we 

have seen that only one in sixteen of its offspring is 

homozygous for any particular combination ; if three 

characters, one in 64, if four characters, one in 256, 

so that it is clear that Mendel’s method of considering 

distinct characters separately must be followed, if 
D. 4) 


66 HEREDITY [cH. 


any rules are to be arrived at for the distribution of 
characters among the offspring of hybrids. 

Before proceeding to consider some of the further 
applications of Mendelian inheritance, a few examples 
will be given of characters in animals and plants 
which are found to be inherited according to this law. 

In plants, flower-colour, seed-colour (due to either 
seed-coat or the contained embryo); production of 
starch or sugar in seeds (maize, see fig. 8 in which both 
forms of seed are shown on the same cob) ; hairiness 
or smoothness (stocks, Lychnis, etc.) ; ‘bearded’ or 
‘beardless’ ears (wheat); ‘palm-leaf’ or ‘fern-leaf’ 
(Primula) ; long or short styles (‘pin-eye’ and ‘thrum- 
eye’ of Primula); pollen-shape, and also fertility or 
sterility of anthers (sweet-pea). Many other examples 
could be given; it should be noted that several of 
these normally occur in nature, e.g. the two flower- 
types of the primrose. 

In animals, coloured coat and albino (many 
mammals); and many other colour-characters in 
mammals and birds ; normal and long or ‘ Angora’ 
hair in rabbit, guinea-pig, etc. (some doubt as to 
completeness of segregation); comb-characters in 
fowls ; leg-feathering in pigeons ; horned and horn- 
less condition (sheep and cattle) ; colour-characters 
in moths, beetles, and snails. In man, several 
abnormal conditions, and presence or absence of 
brown pigment in the iris of the eye. 


v] HEREDITY 67 


As in plants, several of these cases are not in 
any way connected with domestication, and the wide 
diversity of species and characters in which Mendelian 
inheritance has been discovered shows that the phe- 
nomena are not rare or exceptional, but universally 
distributed. 

It has been mentioned that of a pair of allelo- 
morphic characters, one is regarded as containing some 
factor absent from the other, and it may be well to 
give an example of the kind of evidence that leads 

to this conclusion. In fowls there are three chief 
forms of comb; ‘single’ with a median serrated 
ridge, ‘rose’ with a broad upper surface covered 
with papillae, and ‘pea’ with a shape consisting 
essentially of three parallel low ridges. Rose and 
pea each behave as dominants to single, but when 
rose is crossed with pea a fourth type, ‘walnut’ 
results, which in the adult is swollen and dimpled, 
and, in the young at least, is crossed by a transverse 
band of bristles. In the Malay breed such ‘ walnut’ 
combs breed true, but when made by crossing ‘rose’ 
by ‘pea, and mated together, the resulting chicks 
appear in the ratio of 9 walnut, 3 rose, 3 pea, 1 s¢ngle. 
The appearance of singles in the /’, generation from 
pure rose by pure pea is explained by the ‘presence 
and absence’ hypothesis. Rose (2) and pea (P) are 
each allelomorphic with their absence (7, ). A rose- 
combed bird is thus Rp, and a pea-combed rP, 


5-9, 


Fig. 9. Types of combs in Fowls. A. Single Comb (cock). B. Pea 
Comb (cock). C. Pea Comb (hen). D. Rose Comb (cock). 
E. Walnut Comb (young cock). (From Bateson.) 


CH. V] HEREDITY 69 


and the walnut combs produced by crossing them 
have constitution Ri Pp. They produce four kinds 
of germ-cells, RP, Rp, rP, rp, giving the normal 
ratio in F’, of 9 birds containing R and P, 3 with 
Rand p, 3 with r and P,1 rp. This rp, containing 
neither rose nor pea is s¢éngle, which may be regarded 
as the normal comb with no other factor superposed 
upon it. 

In conclusion, one further fact should be noted. 
Although the members of an allelomorphic pair differ 
from each other in that one contains a factor lacking 
in the other, and this present factor is commonly 
dominant over its absence, yet a number of cases are 
known in which the introduction of a factor from one 
parent only is not sufficient to cause its full develop- 
ment in the heterozygote. The crossed offspring are 
then different from both the parental types, and are 
commonly intermediate between them. But when 
such heterozygous forms are mated together or self- 
fertilised, both the homozygous parental types are 
produced in addition to the heterozygous form, as in 
the offspring of a heterozygous tall pea there occur 
homozygous talls and shorts in addition to hetero- 
zygous talls. The classical example of this condition 
is the blue Andalusian fowl. This breed cannot be bred 
true ; when blues are paired together about half the 
chickens are blues and the remainder evenly divided 
between blacks and dirty-whites. By many genera- 


70 HEREDITY [CH v 


tions of selection breeders have tried without success 
to eliminate these black and white ‘wasters, but it 
remained for Bateson and Punnett to show that if 
a black and a white are paired together, only blues 
are produced. The two homozygotes are black and 
white respectively ; when these are paired together 
the single black factor introduced from one parent 
is insufficient to cause the crossed chicks to be full 
black, and a dilute black or ‘blue’ results. Such 
incomplete dominance, in which a single factor 
introduced from one parent is insufficient to bring 
about the same effect in the heterozygote that is 
produced by the ‘double dose’ present in the 
homozygote, has been observed in a number of cases, 
some of which must be referred to later. 


CHAPTER VI 
MENDELIAN HEREDITY (Continued) 
The Inheritance of Colour 


In the simple Mendelian cases discussed in the 
last chapter the separate allelomorphic pairs were 
described as wholly independent of one another, and 
in the manner of their inheritance this description is 
correct for allelomorphic pairs in general except in 
special cases, of which examples will be given later. 
But although allelomorphs of distinct pairs are in- 
herited independently, yet not infrequently they may 
react upon one another so as to give an apparently 
combined effect in the individual bearing them. 
This is especially, but by no means exclusively, 
seen in the colour-characters of animals and plants. 
In the list of examples of Mendelian characters it 
was mentioned that coloured coat in animals or 
coloured flowers in plants behave as an alternative 
to whiteness (albinism, i.e. the absence of pigment). 
But further analysis shows that the appearance of 
colour depends upon the presence of at least two 


72 HEREDITY [cH. 


factors, in the absence of either of which no colour 
is produced. An actual example will make this 
clearer. A white rat is mated with a wild (brown 
or ‘grey’) rat, and since colour dominates over its 
absence the F’, heterozygotes are all grey, like wild 
rats. These grey heterozygotes mated together give 


grey X white 


grey x grey 5, 
1 whit black 
grey x grey F 
GB Pp GB Pp 3 
9 grey 3 black 4 white F 
1 GG PP 1 BB PP 1 GG pp + 
2GB PP 2 BB Cp 2GB pp 
26G Pp e 1 BB pp 
4GB Pp 


coloured and albino in the ratio of three to one. If 
now one of these extracted albinos is mated with 
a black rat, the offspring may not be black but grey, 
and such grey individuals paired together will give 
young in the ratio of 9 grey, 3 black, 4 white. 

The explanation is as follows. For the production 
of colour, two factors must be present, one for the 


vI] HEREDITY 73 


production of pigment in general (P?) and the other 
for the determination of the actual colour of that 
pigment (G=grey, B=black). Neither G nor B 
can produce any visible effect in the absence of P ; 
a rat without P (represented by p) is thus an albino. 
The extracted albinos in F’, from the cross wild grey 
xalbino then contain G derived from their wild 
grandparent. These mated with black give grey 
offspring because grey is dominant! over black, and 
the black individual introduces the factor P which 
was absent in the albino. These grey rats (generation 
F, in the diagram) are thus heterozygous in the pair 
of factors grey and black (G and B) and in the factors 
presence and absence of P (Pandp). They will thus 
produce gametes GP, Gp, BP, Bp, which in meeting 
at random will give 9 zygotes containing G and P, 3 
containing B and P, 3 containing G and p, 1 con- 
taining B and p. But the combinations Gp and Bp, 
not having P, are albinos, and so we get 9 greys, 
3 blacks, 4 whites. 

If in the example just given nothing were known 
of the origin of the white rat which was crossed with 
the black (in the generation marked F’,), it would be 
said that a white variety crossed with a black had 


1 This explanation has been simplified by the omission of the fact 
that G and B do not represent factors for separate pigments, but that 
G consists in the addition of a pigment to hairs already containing B. 
A character dominant in this way is called ‘ epistatic,’ see below p. 75. 


74 HEREDITY (cH. 


produced ‘reversion on crossing’ and the young had 
reverted to the ancestral wild form. It is not of 
course necessary that the albino used to produce 
such a ‘reversion’ should itself be the offspring of 
‘a grey; such grey-bearing albinos may be bred 
together for an indefinite number of generations, 
and still carry the factor G; or if they were ori- 
ginally derived from a black stock they would bear 
the factor B. When such stocks are crossed together 
heterozygous GB albinos are produced, and G and B 
segregate from one another in the albino just as in 
the coloured rats in which the colour-factor P is 
present. The fact that colour in animals and plants 
depends on the concurrent action of distinct factors 
thus explains the phenomena of ‘reversion on crossing’ 
which have so long been a puzzle to biologists. 
Among the varieties of the brown (grey) rat 
only two colour types occur, grey (wild-colour) and 
black, but in the rabbit, mouse and other animals 
more are found. In the mouse there are four funda- 
mental colour-types, yellow, grey, black and chocolate. 
The behaviour of yellow is complicated and not yet 
thoroughly understood, but of the others, grey crossed 
with either black or chocolate gives grey ; black with 
chocolate gives black, and chocolate can only appear 
in the absence of all the others. This was formerly 
described by saying that grey was dominant over 
black and chocolate, and black over chocolate, but 


VI| HEREDITY 75 


this is inconsistent with the hypothesis that allelo- 
morphs exist always in pairs, one possessing a factor 
lacking in the other. More correctly, then, each 
colour is allelomorphic with its absence, but the 
presence of a higher member of the series obscures 
or prevents the development of the lower. This 
is expressed by saying that grey is ‘epistatic’ 
over black and chocolate, and black over choco- 
late. Since chocolate is the lowest member of the 
- series, it has been suggested that its factor is indeed 
the pigment factor represented in the case of the rats 
described above by the symbol P, and that in other 
colours the special factors are present in addition. 
In grey mice yellow, black and chocolate pigments 
are all present in the hairs, but the factor for ‘grey- 
ness’ causes the yellow to be restricted to certain 
parts of the hair. In black mice both black and 
chocolate pigments are present, but the black obscures 
the chocolate, and in chocolate mice this pigment 
alone. is present. 

The object of this rather special digression is to 
show how the hypothesis of a series of colour-factors 
acting together can completely coordinate the pheno- 
mena of colour-inheritance, which very few years ago 
seemed hopelessly confused and subject to no definite 
rules. It is now possible to forecast with accuracy 
the results of a pairing between individuals of different 
colours, if the constitution of the parents with respect 


76 HEREDITY [CH. 


to the colour-factors carried by them is known. Some 
of these cases have been exceedingly difficult to elu- 
cidate because it is often impossible by inspection to 
determine the constitution of a given individual. 
This must be tested by suitable matings with indi- 
viduals of colour lower in the series, and it is then 
found that the results observed ‘agree closely with 
expectation. 

A more surprising instance of ‘reversion on 
crossing’ was discovered by Bateson in sweet-peas. 
He found that within the white variety known as 
‘Emily Henderson’ two distinct types exist, indis- 
tinguishable in appearance, which when crossed 
together give a purple closely resembling the wild 
sweet-pea of Southern Europe. The purple rever- 
sionary form in the first cross, (7), self-fertilised, 
gives in the next generation, (#’,), 9 coloured to 7 
whites. The explanation is that some plants of the 
white form lack one colour factor (called by Bateson 
‘C’); others lack the complementary factor ‘ R,’ 
which if present with C, would produce red pigment. 
Since colour can only appear when both C and # are 
present, each parental form is white, but when crossed 
together C and #& are combined in one plant and 
coloured flowers result. The allelomorphic pairs are 
C and its absence (c), and # and its absence (7°); the 
purple heterozygote is thus Ce Rr, and produces four 
kinds of gametes CR, Cr, cR, cr. These mating at 


VI| HEREDITY 7/7 


random give offspring in the ratio of 9 with C and R, 
3 with ¢ and &, 3 with C and 7, 1 with candy. But 
only those containing both Cand #& can produce colour 
and therefore 9 coloured appear to 7 white. Further, 
among the coloured individuals of F,, both purple 
and red appear, because the factors C and F# together 
produce only red; to get purple a third factor for 
blue (B) must also be present, which can only take 
effect in the presence of both C and R. Since B 
was introduced by one only of the original whites, 
the F', purples were heterozygous for blue as well 
as for C and F (with fully represented constitution 
Cc Rr Bb) and hence among the F’, plants one quarter 
contain no B and in the presence of C’ and F# are 
meda: 


1 In this account, the production of colour (red) is described as 
being due to two factors (C and R&). The recent work of Miss 
Wheldale [42] on the chemical nature of flower-colours indicates 
that the essential bodies are an organic base or ‘chromogen’ and 
an oxidising ferment. The work of Chodat and Bach, however, 
indicates that such oxidising ferments must contain two components, 
neither of which alone is able to oxidise the chromogen and produce 
the coloured derivative—anthocyanin. Both kinds of white sweet- 
pea contain the chromogen, but it seems probable that one component 
of the oxidising ferment is present only in one, and the other com- 
ponent only in the other. Hence no colour can be produced in either. 
But on mating the two whites together, the mechanism for the 
oxidation of the chromogen is again complete, and red colour 
(anthocyanin) is formed, The purple colour (represented by the 
additional ‘factor’ B) is due to a further stage of the oxidation 
of the chromogen than when only red is produced, 


78 HEREDITY [CH. 


In the account given above of the colour-factors 
in the sweet-pea it has been shown that at least two 
separate elements are required to produce colour (in 
this case red), and a third if blue is to be present in 
addition. But for the production of the various shades 
or distribution of colour further factors are known, 
e.g. for the intensification or dilution of colour, and for 
making the wing-petals of the same or different colour 
from the standard. Similar phenomena are concerned 
with colour in animals, of which domestic varieties 
of the rat provide a simple instance. Rats, other 
than albinos, are in general either ‘self-coloured, 
with little or no white (this, if present, is confined 
to the ventral surface), or ‘hooded, i.e. white with 


In some white flowers (snapdragon) experiment shows that the 
chromogen itself may be absent. 

_As the purple colour in sweet-peas is due to more complete 
oxidation of a chromogen than red, so in animals colour-physiologists 
find that the series yellow, brown, black, may represent successive 
oxidation-stages of the same chromogen by the same ferment. The 
various colours of mice, for instance, are not therefore to be regarded 
as necessarily produced by different ferments, but the inherited 
‘colour-factors’ determine to what stage the oxidation of the 
chromogen shall be carried. Some confusion has arisen from 
the assumption that the ‘factors’ postulated by students of heredity 
are actual specific colour-ferments, while they may be rather 
determinants which cause the oxidation of the chromogen to 
proceed to a particular stage, and may be compared with the 
factors which determine the production of a rose-comb or single- 
comb in fowls. 


vt] HEREDITY 79 


coloured head and shoulders and a coloured stripe 
along the spine. The self and hooded factors are an 
allelomorphic pair independent of colour, so that 
a hooded rat may be black or grey. The factors may 
also be borne by albinos, and when very young an 
albino bearing the factor for the hood may be dis- 
tinguished by the different texture of the hair on 
the head and shoulders, giving the appearance of 
a water-mark or ‘ghost-hood. The heterozygote 
_ between self-coloured and hooded patterns differs from 
either parent, being black above and white below— 
the so-called ‘Trish’ type of the fancy. Such ‘ Irish’ 
rats bred together always give both self-coloured and 
hooded rats in addition to Irish among their progeny. 
A similar case in rabbits is that of the well-known 
Dutch marking, which seems to correspond with the 
hooded condition in rats. In flowers the number of 
such characters determining the nature and distribu- 
tion of colour may be considerable, so that among 
the offspring of a cross between two varieties of 
Chinese primulas or snapdragons a very large 
colour-series may be produced, which on first in- 
spection may seem a continuous series from the 
darkest to the palest ; but careful analysis of these 
cases has shown that the different factors may be 
recognised and isolated, and the series of colours 
falls strictly within the rules of Mendelian inherit- 
ance when each factor is considered alone. 


80 HEREDITY [CH. 


Hitherto in discussing the interaction of distinct 
pairs of factors (allelomorphs), colour alone has been 
considered, but cases are known where colour and 
a structural character are interdependent in the ~ 
same way. Interesting examples of this are known 
in stocks and primulas. When a certain smooth- 
leaved cream-flowered stock is crossed with a smooth 
white, the /’, plants are purple and hoary, i.e. they 
revert to the ancestral wild purple and hoary-leaved 
stock. The purple colour appears for the same reason 
that the two forms of white sweet-pea gave purple ; 
one colour-factor is introduced by the white parent 
and its complement by the cream’. But the hoari- 
ness appears because the parents contain a factor for 
hoariness, which can only take effect in plants with 
purple flowers. The parents are therefore smooth 
although they contain the hoary factor. When the 
F’, hoary purples are crossed together, the F’, genera- 
tion consists of purple, white and cream-flowered 
plants in the expected proportions, but only the 
purples are hoary. Smooth-leaved purple strains do 
exist, but these are plants lacking the hoary factor 
altogether ; if it were present, it would appear when- 
ever the flowers contain purple sap. 


1 The cream-colour is due to a quite distinct factor, and the 
pigment is in special bodies (plastids) in the cells of the petals. 
The purple colour is due to a pigment dissolved in the sap, and 
is independent of the cream plastid-colour. 


HEREDITY 


vi] 


Sections of Chinese Primula Flowers 


A. Long-style (‘ pin-eye’). 
B. Short-style (‘ thrum-eye’). 


C. Homostyle. 


Fig. 10. 


82 HEREDITY [CH. 


In Chinese Primulas a curious case of inter- 
relation between flower-colour and structure has 
been investigated by Bateson and Gregory. They 
find that the long-styled and short-styled types of 
flowers, so well known from Darwin’s work, are an 
allelomorphic pair, short-styled being dominant. But 
when the long-styled factor is associated with a con- 
dition in which the yellow eye of the flower is enlarged 
to cover about half the area of the petals, the style 
remains short, although the anthers occupy the typical 
long-styled position in the tube of theflower. This con- 
dition is called ‘homostyle’ (fig. 10 C, p. 81). Whena 
short-styled small-eyed plant iscrossed with homostyle 
large-eyed, all the (F) offspring are short-styled and 
small-eyed, these characters being dominant. But in 
the second generation, (F), obtained by breeding to- 
gether these F’, plants, the following types appear :— 


9 short-style with small eye, 
3 short-style with large eye, 
3 long-style with small eye, 
1 homostyle with large eye. 


The long-styled form has appeared in F,, from short- 
style x homostyle, because homostyle is a condition 
of long-style modified by association with the large 
eye. When this association is broken, the long-style 
appears. 

From these examples of the interaction of distinct 


vI| HEREDITY 83 


allelomorphic pairs, many more of which are now 
known, it will be seen that many of the ‘ exceptions’ 
to the Mendelian rule which have been recorded may 


Fig. 11. Some of the types of flowers in generation F, from the 
cross short-style (thrum) small eye x homostyle large eye. 

A. Long-style, smalleye. B. Homostyle, large eye. C. Short- 
style, small eye. D. Short-style, large eye. 

In A and B the flowers are of the ‘star’ type. This 
character is inherited independently of the style and ‘eye’ 
characters. (From Bateson.) 

6—2 


84 HEREDITY [CH. VI 


be explicable on the assumption that what appears 
to be a simple character is really dependent on two 
or more distinct factors, which become separated on 
crossing with a different form. 

In conclusion, it must be mentioned that a number 
of cases are now known in which a pair of Mendelian 
characters are closely associated with Sex. In some 
cases the sex of the individual determines whether 
a character is dominant or recessive ; for example, if 
a horned race of sheep is crossed with a hornless, the 
male offspring are horned and the females hornless ; 
and in cats, it appears that yellow crossed with black 
gives yellow males but tortoiseshell females. In other 
cases certain Mendelian characters can be borne only 
by germ-cells which will give rise to one or the other 
sex. These two apparently different relations between 
sex and a body character may sometimes be combined ; 
for example in man, colour-blindness is dominant in 
males but recessive in females, but at the same time 
an affected man transmits the ‘factor’ for colour- 
blindness only to his daughters, so that while his 
sons and their descendants are free, his grandsons 
through his daughters may be affected. Lack of 
space forbids a full discussion of these phenomena, 
but they suggest that maleness and femaleness are 
possibly in reality a pair of Mendelian characters, 
inherited in the manner described in the last two 
chapters. 


CHAPTER VII 
SOME DISPUTED QUESTIONS 


In this chapter will be briefly considered certain 
questions which either are still quite unsettled, or 
upon which there is still active disagreement among 
biologists. It will be convenient to take first some 
which are closely connected with the Mendelian theory 
of heredity, and pass on later to others which are 
related equally to any theory of inheritance which 
may be adopted. 

One of the chief lines of attack on the Mendelian 
theory has been the proposition that the absolutely 
complete segregation of allelomorphic characters in 
the germ-cells, postulated by that theory, has not 
been proved. If the theory is rigidly true, then in 
the case of a tall pea crossed with a short (Chap. v) 
the homozygous talls and shorts among the offspring 
of the cross should be as pure for tallness or short- 
ness as the original parents; neither character should 


86 HEREDITY [cH. 


have been influenced in any way by its association 
with the other. It has been maintained that the 
Mendelian categories are not sufficiently definite to 
allow such a statement to be made with certainty. 
The Mendelian can only reply, that in the great 
majority of cases the ‘ extracted’ pure individuals in 
the F, generation do not differ recognisably from 
the original parents in the characters considered, 
and that no signs of impurity can be found in later 
generations. 

There are however instances in which it appears 
that Mendelian segregation may not be perfect. It 
has been maintained that an instance of this is 
provided by hair-length in guinea-pigs. When a 
long-haired (‘Angora’) guinea-pig is mated with 
a short-haired, the F, offspring are short-haired, 
shortness being dominant, owing perhaps to the 
presence of a factor which prevents the growth of 
the hair after reaching a certain length. But when 
such F, (heterozygous) short-hairs were mated 
together, in addition to apparently pure longs and 
shorts, animals with hair of intermediate length 
were produced, and these crossed back with pure 
long-hairs gave no short-haired young. It is suggested 
that the long and short characters have become fused 
in some germ-cells, segregation being incomplete or 
non-existent, so that germ-cells bearing the mixed 
character are produced. Again, in a cross between 


VII] HEREDITY 87 


lop-eared and short-eared rabbits, young with ears of 
intermediate length are produced, and these mated 
together give no evidence of segregation in the next 
generation. From these and some other similar 
observations it must be concluded, either that in 
some cases there is incomplete segregation or even 
complete fusion of alternative characters, or that 
what appear to be simple characters are really com- 
plex, and that the true-breeding intermediates are 


- formed by a new combination of elementary factors. 


An instance, which is perhaps similar, will be men- 
tioned in the next chapter in discussing the inheritance 
of pigment in Man. 

A second question with regard to Mendelian 
segregation has at present received hardly any 
answer, namely, whether the apparently ‘continuous’ 
variations such as were illustrated by the example of 
Johannsen’s beans (Chap. IV) segregate according to 
the Mendelian rule. It was seen that each ‘pure 
line, derived by self-fertilisation from an individual 
plant, has its own type about which the size of the 
beans borne by the plant fluctuates; but it is not 
known whether, when two plants having different 
types (modal sizes) are crossed, segregation takes place 
between the two types in the formation of the germ- 
cells of the crossed individual. To determine this 
would naturally require a long and difficult series of 
experiments, and at present very little is known of 


88 HEREDITY [cH. 


the subject, but when it is remembered how wide- 
spread and in what varied characters segregation has 
been found, it would not seem improbable that it 
should occur in such cases also. 

Another subject which Mendelian investigation 
has brought into prominent notice, and which has led 
to much controversy, is the kind of variation which 
has been effective in the process of evolution. Darwin 
assumed that evolution takes place by the preservation 
of very small ‘ continuous’ variations which occur in 
a direction favourable to the species, but even among 
his immediate followers, for example Huxley, doubt 
was expressed whether larger step-like variations or 
‘mutations’ may not have been operative. Darwin 
rejected this idea chiefly on the ground of the rarity 
of such mutations, which makes it inevitable that the 
mutating individual should generally mate with one 
of the normal type, and so it was supposed that the 
mutation would be diluted and rapidly lost. But 
Mendelian work shows that this dilution does not 
occur in simple cases; the offspring of the cross 
between the mutation and the type produces half its 
germ-cells bearing the mutation to its full extent, 
and these will transmit the mutation until the race 
may become widely infected with it, and not infre- 
quently individuals both of which possess it will mate 
together. If the mutation be dominant, as in the 
case of the well-known black variety of the ‘Peppered 


Vit] HEREDITY 89 


Moth’ (Amphidasys betularia), it may spread rapidly 
until it becomes common, and if recessive it will 
equally often be represented in the germ-cells of 
many individuals and will appear when two which 
bear it mate together. In either case if the mutation 
be advantageous it may be preserved at the expense 
of the type by natural selection, until it obtains a 
firm footing. But the difficulty has naturally been 
felt that the marvellously perfect adaptations which 
are so frequent in nature cannot be imagined to 
have arisen by large steps, but must have been ac- 
quired gradually, and therefore many naturalists 
reject the suggestion that mutations can have been 
largely operative in evolution. But the fallacy here 
is the assumption that all discontinuous variations 
must be large ; the case of Johannsen’s beans shows 
that essentially stable variations occur, which pro- 
bably differ from mutations only in their small extent, 
and by the selection of such ‘minute mutations’ 
the wonderfully perfect structures of living things 
might be produced. It may perhaps be regarded as 
hair-splitting to distinguish between minute mutations 
and fluctuating variability, but the distinction lies in 
the nature of their inheritance, which is the essential 
thing in evolution. It has been seen that no selection 
within the ‘pure line’ in the case of the beans has 
any effect ; for progress to be made, a new mutation, 
small though it might be, is necessary. 


90 HEREDITY [CH. 


The problem in heredity which has probably 
given rise to more controversy than any other is that 
alluded to more than once previously, of the inherit- 
ance or non-inheritance of acquired characters, that 
is, characters produced in the individual during its 
life by the action of some sort of stimulus. Some 
aspects of the question have already been considered, 
and from what has been shown of the very definite 
nature of the inheritance of germinal (inborn) cha- 
racters, it will be understood why students of heredity 
are increasingly disposed, @ priori, to disbelieve in 
the transmission of acquirements; for if these were 
transmitted to any considerable extent, this fact 
must interfere, one would suppose, with the orderly 
appearance in the offspring of the characters repre- 
sented in the germ-cells of the parents. But at the 
present time no treatment of heredity could be re- 
garded as complete without some mention of the evi- 
dence which has been adduced in favour of the 
transmission of such characters. Unfortunately, the 
evidence is almost always capable of interpretation 
in more than one sense. The supporters of the belief 
in transmission rely largely on indirect evidence, 
especially on the difficulty of imagining any cause 
of evolution in certain directions if the effects of 
acquirement are excluded. A vast literature has 
grown up around this question, of which only 
illustrative examples can be given. In animals which 


vit] HEREDITY 91 


live in the dark the pigment in the skin is frequently 
absent, as it is also in flat-fish on the side of the body 
which lies protected from light on the sea-floor. It is 
said that pigment in such cases cannot be harmful, and 
so its disappearance is not due to natural selection. 
But pigment very generally appears in response to 
the action of light, and so it is supposed that the 
absence of the stimulus to production, acting for 
many generations, has caused the pigment to dis- 
appear. This is illustrated by the well-known 
experiment of Cunningham on flat-fish. The young 
fish is pigmented on both sides of the body; it 
then settles on one side and the pigment on that 
side disappears. Cunningham reared such young 
fish in an aquarium lighted from below: when they 
settled on the bottom the pigment disappeared, but 
if kept still longer exposed to light from beneath, 
the pigment began to come back again. The dis- 
appearance of the pigment, although exposed to 
light, proves that the loss is hereditary ; its return 
on continued exposure to light is interpreted by 
Cunningham to mean that its disappearance was 
due to absence of light, and has gradually become 
hereditary, but that the process can be reversed by 
again exposing the lower side to the action of the 
stimulus. The same argument has been used with 
regard to the colourless skins and vestigial eyes of 
animals living in caves; where the structure is use- 


92 HEREDITY (CH. 


less it disappears, and the most obvious cause to 
assume is lack of use, which, acting cumulatively 
through many generations, has become hereditary. 
Such evidence, however, is only presumptive, it does 
not amount to proof, and on the other side may be 
adduced the pigments of birds’ eggs. Birds which 
nest in holes or dark places usually have colourless 
or slightly coloured eggs, while those which lay in 
open places have eggs more or less matching their 
surroundings. This appears closely comparable with 
the condition of skin-colour in fishes and amphibians, 
and yet it is impossible that the action of light could 
have any direct effect on the production of pigment 
in birds’ eggs. That the loss of pigment in each case 
is connected with its uselessness is probable, but the 
birds’ egg case seems to show that it is not due to 
‘use-inheritance.’ 

A second instance of the indirect evidence for the 
inheritance of acquired characters may be given, that 
of instinct. Instincts are very similar to firmly rooted 
habits, and have been regarded as habits which from 
being performed through many generations have 
become hereditary. There can be no doubt that, in 
the higher animals especially, instincts may be rein- 
forced and perfected by habit, but many cases can be 
adduced in which it seems impossible that habit has 
played a part in the evolution of an instinct. Many 
insects have exceedingly perfect and complex in- 


VIt] HEREDITY 93 


stincts in connexion with egg-laying, yet the process 
may last only a few hours, and the eggs may all be 
fully formed and ready for laying before the insect 
hatches. In the worker-bee, too, there are many 
admirably developed instincts, and also structural 
features which might be thought to have originated 
by the transmission of acquired adaptations, and yet 
the worker-bee, except in rare cases, never repro- 
duces itself, but is produced by a queen and a drone 
with structures and instincts different from its own. 
If in these cases we find perfect instincts which 
cannot have arisen by the inheritance of acquire- 
ments, it seems unreasonable to assume that instincts 
in other species must have arisen in this way. These 
two cases are given merely as examples of the pre- 
sumptive evidence that has been brought forward. 
It is admitted that the process of evolution would 
be more easily comprehensible if the inheritance 
of acquired characters were a fact, but it is clear 
that no absolute proof of its existence can be based 
on cases of this kind. 


Exact experiments on the possible inherited 
effects of acquirements are difficult to devise so as 
to be unequivocal, and most have given negative 
results. Experiments on butterflies have been men- 
tioned in Chapter 11, so further reference to them is 
not required. A case which at first sight seems to 


94 HEREDITY [CH. 


prove the inherited effects of conditions is the 
experiment of Kellogg in starving silkworms, in 
which he found that when the caterpillars were 
starved for two generations, the third generation, 
even if well fed, were below the normal size. But 
there is here a possible source of error, that the eggs 
produced by starved females may have been lacking 
in yolk, so that the resulting caterpillars would be 
weakly from the beginning and never overtake the 
normal size. If so, the apparent effect of mherit- 
ance of bad conditions would be due really to poor 
embryonic nourishment, not to germinal difference. 
The same explanation might apply to the apparent 
cumulative effects of under-feeding in man, if the 
mother cannot adequately nourish the infant before 
birth. The famous experiments of Brown-Séquard 
on the inheritance of artificial injuries in guinea-pigs 
must be mentioned. He found that when the parents 
were subjected to operations of various kinds, some 
of the young showed corresponding abnormalities, 
especially in the case of the effects of certain injuries 
to the nervous system. Subsequent experiments 
however have not completely confirmed his results, 
and there is reason to believe that where they have 
been confirmed there are other possible explanations 
of the apparent transmission of the effects of injury. 
For example, Brown-Séquard found that when the 
chief nerve of the leg is severed, the toes become 


Vit] HEREDITY 95 


morbid and the animals frequently nibble them away. 
A small percentage of the offspring of guinea-pigs 
lacking toes from this cause also had toes missing. 
But it has been pointed out that rodents in captivity 
sometimes eat off the toes or tails of their young, and 
if the mother had acquired the habit of nibbling her 
own toes, she might bite off those of her young 
shortly after birth and give the appearance of the 
inheritance of a mutilation [20, 29]. 

Quite recently Kammerer [19]in Vienna has made 
some remarkable observations on salamanders and 
a species of toad which seem to support the idea of 
the inheritance of acquired characters. For example, 
among other experiments, he finds that the animals 
can be accustomed to lay their eggs in water instead 
of on land, and the young become modified to suit 
their new surroundings, and the modifications are 
_ progressively increased in later generations. He 
points out however that most of his results, like 
those obtained by Tower (Chap. 111), may have been 
brought about by the action of environment on the 
eggs at the time of maturation, but they differ from 
Tower's in the regularity of their appearance and in 
being adaptive. Further work in this direction will 
be awaited with interest. 

On the whole, the hypothesis of the inheritance 
of acquired characters must be regarded as ‘not 
proven, and our increasing knowledge of the be- 


96 HEREDITY [CH. 


haviour of germinal characters makes it improbable 
that it can be a factor of great importance in the 
constitution of the individual, or to the course of 
evolution. Some further evidence in this direction 
will be given in the next chapter. 

A few minor questions remain. One of these, 
which has played a considerable part in biological 
literature, is the alleged phenomenon called Telegony. 
It was formerly believed, and the belief is still firmly 
held by fanciers and animal breeders, that if a female 
of one breed bears young by a male of another breed, 
and is then mated with a male of her own kind, the 
offspring of this second mating will in some cases 
show the influence of the first sire, and instead of 
being pure-bred will in some respects be mongrels 
resembling the mongrel offspring of the first mating. 
The instance of this made classical by Darwin is 
‘Lord Morton’s Mare,’ in which a chestnut mare bore 
a colt by a quagga, and afterwards two colts by a 
black Arab stallion, both of which were dun-coloured, 
and bore stripes on the legs and in one colt on the 
neck also [7]. But it is known that dun horses are 
frequently striped to some extent, and LEwart’s 
well-known work with zebras [11], in which it was 
attempted to repeat this experiment, gave negative 
results. The belief in telegony is widely held among 
dog-fanciers, and many cases could be quoted, but 
whenever properly controlled experiments have been 


Vit] HEREDITY 97 


made, no evidence of telegony has been forthcoming. 
The belief in it is almost certainly due to the habit 
of generalising from individual instances ; whenever 
a case occurs which appears to favour the belief, it is 
adduced as proof, even though other causes may have 
been operative, and matings in which no evidence for 
it appears are passed over in silence. If it were 
a genuine phenomenon, it is almost certain that 
conclusive evidence for it would have been obtained 
in the numerous breeding experiments recorded in 
recent years. 


Another idea very widely held, but apparently 
resting on no better evidence, is the belief in maternal 
impressions, especially in the case of mankind. By 
maternal impression is meant the influencing of the 
child by events affecting the mother during pregnancy, 
It is commonly believed that if a pregnant woman 
is injured in any part, or even sees an injury to 
another person so as to excite her imagination, the — 
corresponding part in the child may be abnormally 
developed, or may bear some mark, caused, it is 
supposed, by an impression conveyed from the 
mother. More general still is the belief that the 
temperament of the child is influenced by the mother’s 
mental condition during pregnancy. This latter belief 
is scarcely susceptible of accurate investigation, but 
the belief in bodily marks or malformations being 

D. 7 


98 HEREDITY [CH. VII 


due to corresponding injury to the mother, or to her 
attention being strongly attracted to that part, is 
almost certainly based on coincidence. A_ large 
number of children are born with some abnormality, 
and a very large proportion have some mark on the 
skin. Many mothers during pregnancy undergo some 
slight accident or see some deformed person, and thus 
it must happen that a mark on the child will often 
roughly coincide in position with the part affected in 
the parent. If every coincidence of this kind is 
quoted as proof of the reality of maternal impression, 
and the cases are left unheeded in which no relation 
can be found between abnormality in the child and 
events affecting the mother, it is natural that a 
belief in the phenomenon will easily take firm root. 
The evidence available however is probably insuffi- 
cient to support any other view than that of accidental 


coincidence. 


CHAPTER VIII 
HEREDITY IN MAN 


In the chapters dealing with the various aspects 
of heredity in general, a number of instances have 
been given illustrating inheritance of various cha- 
racters in man, and the province of this concluding 
chapter will be to collect and add to these cases, so 
as to sketch the general outlines of what is known 
of human inheritance. It has been seen that as man 
differs in no important way in his bodily characters 
from the other mammalia, so the laws governing the 
variation and transmission of those characters are 
the same as are found throughout the animal and 
vegetable kingdoms wherever they have been in- 
vestigated ; and further that the ‘mental and moral’ 
attributes of man, which presumably are correlated 
with physical structures, are inherited no less strongly 
than the bodily features themselves. When investi- 
gated by the biometric methods, the stature, span, 
length of fore-arm, eye-colour, and certain other 
physical characters or measurements, are found to 


7—2 


100 HEREDITY [CH. 


give a parental correlation and thus an intensity of 
inheritance closely similar to those obtained from 
the study of various animals and plants. When 
various non-measurable and less definite characters 
such as intellectual ability, hand-writing, etc. are 
investigated by the same methods, a similar intensity 
of heredity is found, and finally the same is true when 
the character chosen is liability to certain diseases, 
notably tuberculosis and insanity, or such abnormal 
conditions as congenital deafness. Since these latter 
conditions have been only briefly alluded to, and are 
of such fundamental importance for the well-being 
of mankind, the evidence may be referred to rather 
more fully here. The case of insanity is especially 
convincing, for it is not open to the objection some- 
times made with regard to infectious diseases that 
the cause of the apparent inheritance of the condition 
is the exposure of the child to infection from the 
parent. But it must be remembered that there are 
many kinds of insanity, in one of which at least 
(chorea), the inheritance appears to be Mendelian, 
and that of two men with equal tendency to mental 
aberration, one who is not exposed to strain may 
remain normal through life, while another under 
more arduous conditions may break down. But the 
data of occurrence of insanity among tainted stocks 
make it certain that ‘the insane diathesis is inherited 
with at least as great an intensity as any physical or 


VIII | HEREDITY 101 


mental character in man. It forms...probably no 
exception to an orderly system of inheritance in 
man, whereby on the average about one-half of the 
mean parental character, whether physical, mental or 
pathological, will be found in the child. It is accord- 
ingly highly probable that it is in the same manner 
as other physical characters capable of selection or 
elimination by unwise or prudential mating in the 
course of two or three generations. (Heron [10)). 
Similarly for congenital deafness, Schuster writes 
‘...that striking confirmation has been obtained of 
previous work on widely different characters, at any 
rate with regard to the correlation between father 
and children, and mother and children.’ [31]. 
These examples, which might be added to, of 
results obtained by ‘biometric’ methods, make it 
sufficiently clear that a knowledge of the facts of 
inheritance is of importance to mankind, and that 
the further collection of accurate data is one of the 
most needed social requirements. Before passing on 
to other aspects of the question one other subject 
may be mentioned. The measure of resemblance 
in these characters has not only been worked out 
between parent and child, but between brothers 
and sisters, between children and grandparents and 
uncles and aunts, and between cousins. Some esti- 
mate can therefore be made of the probability of an 
individual being affected if his relatives are known, 


102 HEREDITY (CH. 


a thing which should not only be useful to insurance 
offices, but to all thinking men, for it may ultimately 
become the basis for deciding on the propriety of 
marriage by members of tainted families. In general 
it appears that the resemblance of a child to its grand- 
parent is rather more than half of that to its parent, 
and that the resemblance between uncle and nephew, 
or between first cousins, is very slightly less than 
between grandchild and grandparent. 

We may now turn to definitely discontinuous cha- 
racters in man, some of which are clearly Mendelian 
in their inheritance. One of the most interesting 
cases is that of eye-colour. Hurst [17j has shown that 
complete absence of pigment in the front of the iris | 
is recessive to the presence of pigment; that is to 
say, that two pure blue-eyed people have only blue- 
eyed offspring, but that a blue-eyed individual married 
to one with any brown or yellow in the iris may have 
children with pigmented eyes and that two pigmented 
parents have pigmented children, with or without 
a proportion of blue-eyed in addition. Within the 
pigmented class there is great range of variation, 
from a small yellow rim round the pupil to com- 
pletely dark eyes, and the relation of the various 
pigmented types to each other has not yet been 
analysed. But that the characters ‘pigmented’ and 
‘non-pigmented’ are a Mendelian pair, his evidence 
leaves no doubt. This is thus a case in which the 


VIII | HEREDITY 103 


occurrence of apparently continuous variation within 
a discontinuous category is clearly shown. Of other 
Mendelian characters in man, colour-blindness, com- 
plicated by its relation with sex, has already been 


Fig. 12. Brachydactylous hands. (From Bateson, after Farabee.) 


mentioned. Several cases are known in which an 
abnormality behaves as a simple dominant, e.g. the 
condition of the fingers known as ‘ brachydactyly, in 


104 HEREDITY [CH. 


which the fingers have one joint less than the normal ; 
congenital cataract, and probably other diseases of 
the eye. Perhaps the most remarkable human 
pedigree ever collected is one of ‘night-blindness, 
extending through nine generations and going back to 
the seventeenth century, which has been published by 
Nettleship (see [1]). The condition is one in which 
the patient cannot see in dull light, and it behaves 
as a Mendelian dominant, probably, however, with 
some complication, since the numbers affected are less 
than the theoretical expectation. In all these cases 
in which the abnormality is dominant, only affected 
individuals can transmit it; the normal members of 
the family have only normal offspring, a condition 
which is shortly summarised as ‘once free, always 
fees 

But the rule that the affected alone transmits will 
be followed only when the condition depends on a 
single factor ; if it depends on more than one, or if 
its dominance is modified by sex or other conditions, 
then non-affected individuals may have affected off- 
spring. This is possibly the case in many diseases in 
which it appears that the affection is dominant, and 
yet certain non-affected individuals have affected 
offspring, and in such examples it must also be 
remembered that the disease is probably not always 
developed in people in whom the tendency is present ; 
the tendency may be there but the conditions required 


105 


HEREDITY 


VIII | 


neocons 
. ai} + ©+— © O 
se ‘se! (e—o@ ae 
oft . 
oot 
of * 
10) ® © 
rete 
‘e & to) 
Ly 8 


e Affected male 


os Unaffected male 


e Affected female 


2 Unaffected female 


3 females 
© 8 individuals, sex not recorded 


2 


| Not certainly affected 


e 
? 


—— Indicates consanguinity. 


106 HEREDITY (CH. 


to bring out the disease may be avoided, especially if 
it is a condition not present at birth, but appearing 
later in life. This kind of thing may perhaps be 
illustrated by the pedigrees of Retinitis pigmentosa 
taken from Nettleship (Bowman Lecture, 1909 [23]) 
on p. 105. 

The disease is not usually present at birth, but 
comes on at a varying age, sometimes during or after 
middle life, and it will be seen that in the first 
pedigree it is transmitted only by affected members 
of the family, so behaving as a typical Mendelian 
dominant. In the second pedigree, however, it com- 
monly ‘skips’ a generation, the parents of affected 
individuals usually being normal, but themselves 
children or sisters of those who are affected. The fact 
that in both families (as in most cases of this disease) 
males are more frequently affected than females, 
suggests that there is some complication, and this 
is perhaps connected with the fact that in one family 
the disease behaves as a simple dominant, while in 
the other it is most frequently transmitted, like 
colour-blindness, through normal females from affected 
males. These pedigrees are given as examples of 
the somewhat irregular inheritance of diseased con- 
ditions such as may frequently be seen in the medical 
journals ; many of them are probably explicable in 
the ways suggested above. 

A somewhat different group of phenomena is 


vit] HEREDITY 107 


illustrated by the inheritance of pigmentation in 
man, in skin- and hair-colour. In the case of hair- 
colour, Hurst has given evidence that bright red 
behaves as a recessive to dark-coloured hair, and 
that to some extent at least segregation takes place. 
But the shades of hair-colour graduate into one 
another so continuously that it is impossible to 
place them with confidence in Mendelian categories, 
and further the colour alters so greatly between 
infancy and maturity in many persons that classifi- 
cation is difficult. Many children for example have 
bright red hair, in whom during adolescence the 
colour deepens to brown, while other members of 
the same family, whose hair has hardly differed from 
the first during childhood, keep the bright red until 
middle life. In families with red hair we may see 
clear evidence of segregation between red and dark 
brown hair-colour, but the differences between the 
originally red-haired individuals show that some 
contain a darkening factor which is absent in the 
others. Probably then in human hair-colour there 
are a number of factors which interact upon one 
another in a way even more complex than in the 
hair-colour of mice mentioned in a previous chapter ; 
and where rigid experiment is impossible, the analysis 
of these factors is almost hopeless. The same remarks 
perhaps apply to the various eye-colours within the 
class with pigmented iris, and the very frequent but 


108 HEREDITY (CH. 


not absolutely perfect correlation between dark eye- 
colour and dark hair suggests that similar factors 
may perhaps be at work in both cases. 

The inheritance of skin-colour in man is also one 
of the cases which has hitherto defied Mendelian 
analysis, and has been quoted more than once as 
disproving the universality of Mendelian inheritance. 
When a ‘white’ European marries a negro, the off- 
spring are ‘mulattoes, intermediate between the 
parents. Mulattoes however are not all alike, some 
have brown skins and some yellowish. When they 
marry among themselves they are said never to 
produce full blacks or full whites, but again mulat- 
toes, who however vary in the depth of their colour. 
When a mulatto marries a white, the ‘quadroon’ 
offspring are lighter than mulattoes but usuaily 
darker than Europeans ; there is evidence however 
that they vary considerably, with possibly a certain 
amount of discontinuity between the darkest and 
lightest. Some evidence of segregation is also pro- 
vided by the occasional instances of ‘throw back’ 
to very dark skin and negroid features or hair among 
children of apparently white people with some negro 
ancestry. The whole problem however is very insuffi- 
ciently known, and the difficulty of obtaining reliable 
data is doubtless increased by race-prejudice. Taken 
in mass, the results of crossing white and black races 
seem to give a blended inheritance with continuous 


VIII | HEREDITY 109 


variation ; but as has been seen in the case of hair- 
colour the accurate investigation of individual families 
would possibly show that several factors were con- 
cerned, and that in the later generations, only when 
all these factors are combined in one individual 
would the colour be identical with that of either of 
the original races. In this respect crosses between 
different races of mankind resemble hybrids between 
different species of animals or plants, except that 
there is usually no sterility. Most of the Mendelian 
investigations have been made on varieties which 
differ in few characters, for the sake of simplicity, 
but when species are crossed and the offspring are 
fertile so many diverse characters are concerned, of 
which the relation to one another is not generally 
known, that the offspring of the hybrids may con- 
tain no individuals closely resembling either parent 
species. This has been explained by saying that only 
varietal and not specific characters segregate from 
one another on the Mendelian scheme, but it is not 
improbably due to the multiplicity of characters con- 
cerned, and their complicated interrelations, which 
makes analysis exceedingly difficult. It is also 
not impossible, when germ-cells differing very con- 
siderably in constitution combine in fertilisation, 
that in the formation of the germ-cells of the next 
generation the machinery for segregation is inade- 
quate. xtreme cases of this are possibly the cause 


110 HEREDITY [CH. 


of the frequent sterility of hybrids, but it may be 
that when the parental differences are insufficient to 
prevent the formation of fertile germ-cells, they may 
yet be enough to interfere with normal Mendelian 
segregation. 

Certain aspects of inheritance in mankind have 
now been reviewed, and it remains briefly to indicate 
the lines on which our knowledge may be of practical 
importance. One of the things which is especially 
prominent when the evidence is considered as a 
whole is the exceeding definiteness or determinancy 
of the process of heredity. Given parents of certain 
constitution, it can be said with confidence that on 
the average a certain proportion of their offspring 
will have such and such characters. It matters not 
whether the character considered is regarded from 
the standpoint of the Biometrician or the Mendelian, 
both agree that what is present in the germ-cell will 
be present in the individual, and that external con- 
ditions as a rule play but a small part in determining 
its appearance. The Biometrician finds an average 
value for the intensity of inheritance, and shows that 
it is sensibly the same whether the character con- 
sidered is stature, eye-colour, ability, or tendency to 
congenital disease. When the character in question 
is a simple case of presence or absence, the Men- 
delian finds that it is present in a definite proportion 
of the children of affected parents, so that he can say 


VIII | HEREDITY EES 


with confidence that among the offspring of a parent 
who has congenital cataract or abnormally jointed 
fingers, about one-half will be similarly affected, and 
there is no hope in such a case that the severity 
of the affection will diminish in later generations. 
Where the disease depends on several factors, it 
may perhaps be eliminated by repeated marriage 
with untainted stock, but in such cases as cataract 
or colour-blindness there is no hope of this. 

It is commonly supposed that inherited disease 
arises largely from the cumulative effect of bad 
conditions, drink and the like, but it has been seen 
how doubtful it is whether the effects of such things 
are really transmitted, and in any case it can be 
proved that in comparison with the germinal con- 
stitution, the effects of environment are relatively 
insignificant. Galton was one of the first to illustrate 
this by the study of twins. Human twins are of two - 
sorts; in one case they arise by the simultaneous 
development of two ova, as in the litters of lower 
animals, and then they are no more alike than other 
children of the same parents, and may be of different 
sexes. Twins of the second kind are probably pro- 
duced by division of one ovum, and are then of the 
same sex and so alike as to be called ‘identical.’ 
Such ‘identical’ twins remain through life, despite 
differences of environment, more like one another 
than successively born brothers commonly are, even 


112 HEREDITY [CH. 


when brought up in precisely the same surroundings. 
The same thing has been shown by an investigation of 
school-children in relation to their home environment 
and the habits of their parents. From a study of over 
70,000 children in Glasgow, classified according to the 
employment or non-employment of their mothers in 
work outside the home, it was found that the rela- 
tion of their height and weight to the employment or 
non-employment of the mother was almost negligible 
compared with the relation between the physical 
characters of the mother and child. Still more 
surprising, if correct, is the observation that no 
regular relation could be found between drinking 
habits in the parents and the health, intelligence 
or physical development of some 1400 children in 
the schools of Edinburgh. [Elderton, 10.]  Investi- 
gations of this kind are still in their infancy, and 
perhaps more urgently needed than any other social 
data—and it would be rash to make sweeping general 
statements from the little that has been done. 
Results like the examples quoted make one doubt 
whether the generally accepted statements about the 
degeneration caused by unhealthy conditions or drink 
are really at all reliable. It is easy where insanity 
or other disease occurs, to say that in so many per 
cent. of the cases there has been alcoholism in the 
ancestors, and that therefore alcoholism is a cause of 
insanity ; but in the first place it must be shown that 


“viit] HEREDITY 113 


the alcoholism is not the result of nervous disorder, 
which in the next generation appears as insanity ; 
and in the second place, in order to prove a causal 
connexion, in addition to this it must be shown that 
insanity is actually more frequent in the descendants 
of drunkards than in those of the sober. The un- 
doubted evils of excessive drinking are many and 
obvious enough, but it does not follow that physical 
or mental degeneration of the descendants are among 
them, and it may be a false hope to suppose that 
these evils could be removed merely by the abolition 
of drink. 

The same sort of argument may apply to the 
undoubted physical and mental inferiority of our 
slum population. It is not yet proved whether this 
is the effect of miserable surroundings, or whether 
the ‘unfit’ gravitate to the worst places because the 
more fit occupy the better. These are problems 
which society has as yet scarcely attempted to face, 
and yet it is clear that on their correct solution 
depends the central question of social reform. If 
man is to any appreciable extent the creature of his 
environment, then improved conditions will improve 
the race. But if, as the study of heredity suggests, 
though it would be rash to say it is proved, man 
is almost entirely the product of inborn factors 
which are hardly affected by environment, then 
improved conditions may only encourage the pro- 

D. 8 


114 HEREDITY (cH. 


pagation of the degenerate, and the race as a 
whole may go back rather than forward. Respon- 
sible students are not lacking who maintain that this 
is already taking place. It is said that the increase 
of insanity which is believed to have taken place 
in modern times is due to the provision of asylums 
where the insane are properly cared for and fre- 
quently discharged as ‘cured.’ When the insane 
were treated on the ‘straight jacket’ system no cure 
could be effected, and so the unfortunates could not 
recover to propagate their kind. But on the present 
system it not infrequently happens that the insane 
are enabled to bring into the world large families, so 
that it is not improbable that the increase in number 
may be due to this, rather than to the increased 
strain of modern conditions. No one would advocate 
a return to the old system, but some restriction 
on the reproduction of the mentally deficient is 
undoubtedly demanded by modern knowledge of 
heredity. 

It is even said that hospitals and the feeding of 
destitute school-children are really working in the 
direction opposite to what is intended, by enabling 
the degenerate to live and beget families, who under 
harder conditions would never have survived’. If a 


1 It is of course not suggested that all or even the majority of 
those who receive such help are degenerate, but it can hardly be 


doubted that a very high proportion of the ‘ unfit’ will take advan- 
tage of it. 


VIII | HEREDITY Lis 


child is to survive it is undoubtedly better that he 
should be well fed and cared for, but looking at the 
matter apart from all sentiment, it is quite possible 
that posterity will be worse rather than better as a 
result of such institutions. It is not improbable that 
future generations will find that our methods for the 
relief of distress are on wrong lines, and that other 
means must be found for dealing with the problem, 
which will cure the evil at its root instead of attempt- 
ing to alleviate the symptoms. 

Another point at which the study of heredity 
touches social problems is the treatment of criminals. 
It is becoming recognised that a large proportion of 
criminals are In some way abnormal, and that their 
crimes are due not to evil surroundings nor to wilful 
perversity, but to inherited defects. If this is 
actually the case, penal treatment of such is no less 
cruel than similar treatment of the insane, but in 
both cases efforts at reclamation or cure, followed 
by liberty and encouragement te marry, may only 
lead to a repetition of the same evils in the next 
generation. The present teaching of biology is per- 
fectly clear, that in the case of the evils mentioned 
above and many others, marriage of those afflicted, 
and to a less extent of their near relatives, involves 
a grave risk of transmitting the affection to descen- 
dants, and so of inflicting serious injury upon society’. 


1 See [43]. 
8—2 


APPENDIX I 


HISTORICAL SUMMARY OF THEORIES 
OF HEREDITY 


In the foregoing pages, Heredity has been regarded 
as the relation between parents and offspring in 
respect of their bodily characters, and it has been 
shown that this relation has been brought about in 
some way by the germ-cells, but very little has been 
said about the mechanism by which this is effected. 
This side of the question is very largely speculative, 
and in order to keep speculation to some extent 
apart from facts, an account of theories of hereditary 
transmission, and of recent work on the supposed 
material basis of hereditary characters, has been 
postponed to appendices, which the reader who seeks 
for facts and well-founded deductions alone, may 
omit at will. First, a summary of the chief theories 
of heredity will be given, and then a short account of 
recent work on the subject. 

In the introduction it was pointed out how closely 
related are our ideas of Heredity and Variation with 


21 a ELE MEY aca 117 


theories of Evolution, and thus the history of the two 
subjects largely coincides. The first important theory 
was that of Lamarck, published in 1809, and although 
it had little influence at the time, in more recent 
years Lamarck’s main principle has found many sup- 
porters. His theory was essentially that ‘acquired ’ 
modifications are being continually produced and 
perfected by every organism during its life, and that 
they are at least partially transmitted to its offspring, 
so that each generation will be rather better adapted 
to its surroundings than its predecessor. In this way, 
for example, the great length of the neck of the 
giraffe would be explained by the continual striving 
through many generations to reach higher leaves on 
the trees; or the limbless condition of snakes and 
slow-worms by the gradual loss of limbs through 
disuse. But it has been seen that the assumption 
that acquired characters are inherited is open to 
grave doubt, and hence the followers of Lamarck 
are fewer at the present time than formerly. 
Darwin’s great theory of Evolution by Natural 
Selection of course depends on quite different prin- 
ciples, but it, like Lamarck’s, is based essentially upon 
the laws of variation and heredity. Darwin himself 
made astonishing progress in the investigation of 
these laws, and although he would doubtless have 
been the first to admit the incompleteness of our 
knowledge, yet he collected sufficient evidence to 


118 HEREDITY [ AP. 


enable him to formulate one of the first really impor- 
tant theories of heredity, which he called the Theory 
of Pangenesis [7, (1868)]. The essence of his theory 
was that every cell of an organism gives off minute 
particles or ‘Gemmules’ from itself, which circulate 
in the body and finally come to rest in the germ-cells, 
or in parts where buds may be developed. The 
gemmules were regarded as being capable of multi- 
plication, and of transmission to a future generation 
in a dormant state. They were supposed to be given 
off from all tissues at every stage of development, so 
that every unit of the organism at every stage would 
be represented in the germ-cells. On the develop- 
ment of the germ-cell, the contained gemmules 
would give rise to cells like those from which they 
were derived, and so the characters of one generation 
would be transmitted to those which follow. 

By this hypothesis Darwin accounted for the 
phenomena of sexual and non-sexual reproduction, 
regeneration of lost parts, variability, inheritance 
both of inborn and acquired characters, and lastly 
of reversion to a previous ancestor. The hypothesis 
was one of the first which attempted to bring all 
these various groups of facts into line, but it had 
the serious defect that there was no direct evidence 
whatever for the existence of gemmules, and, assuming 
their existence, to be accommodated in the germ-cells 
they must be so exceedingly minute as to be almost 


T| HEREDITY 119 


unimaginable. The Theory of Pangenesis never gained 
any very wide acceptance, but is of great importance 
owing to its stimulating effect on later work and 
thought. To a great extent it led to the formulation 
of other theories of heredity’, any account of which is 
prevented by limitation of space. It can only be 
mentioned that the chief hypotheses which followed 
Darwin’s laid successively more and more emphasis 
upon the idea that the germ-cells are not made up of 
samples taken from the body, but have a certain 
independence. So grew up the conception of ‘germinal 
continuity, that is, the idea that the germ-cell of one 
generation gives rise not only to the body of the next, 
but also directly to its germ-cells, so that the body 
does not produce germ-cells, but only contains them. 
We must now turn to the theory in which this idea finds 
its most celebrated expression, Weismann’s Theory 
of the Germ-plasm (1885) [40, 8]. 

It is impossible in a short space to give an 
adequate account of Weismann’s great theory, which 
he has worked out in fuller minuteness of detail than 
has been done with any other theory of heredity, and 
by which he has done more to stimulate discussion 
and research than perhaps any biologist since Darwin. 


1 For a summary of the more important theories of heredity, 
especially those of Herbert Spencer (1863, i.e. before Darwin’s theory 
of Pangenesis), Galton (1875) and de Vries (1889), see Thomson’s 
Heredity [33]. See also [8]. 


120 HEREDITY [ AP. 


Weismann was led by his work on the origin of the 
germ-cells to a belief in germinal continuity as 
explained above, but the facts of regeneration of 
lost parts and other related phenomena caused him 
to give up the idea that a sharp distinction could be 
drawn between the cells of the body and the germ, 
and to substitute for it the idea of a distinction 
between body-substance and germ-substance, or as 
he calls it, body-plasm and germ-plasm. According 
to this hypothesis, the egg contains germ-plasm 
derived from that of the parent, and as the egg 
develops the germ-plasm increases and becomes 
distributed among the cells, and gradually, as the 
cells become specialised to form the different parts, 
the germ-plasm becomes converted into body-plasm 
and builds up the varied kinds of cells of the 
body. But some cells continue to possess the full 
complement of ancestral germ-plasm, and these will 
go to form the germ-cells of the next generation. 
When an organ remains capable of regenerating 
lost parts, it is assumed that germ-plasm having 
the power to develop such parts remains in the cells 
and becomes active when required. Germ-plasm can 
thus be converted into body-plasm, but body-plasm 
cannot become germ-plasm, and hence Weismann 
assumes that no change brought about m the body 
(by environment, etc.) but not affecting the germ-cells, 
can be inherited by subsequent generations. It is 


T] HEREDITY _ 121 


therefore impossible according to his theory, that 
‘acquired characters’ in the technical sense should 
ever be inherited. The germ-plasm of one genera- 
tion gives origin to the germ-plasm of the next, and 
no external conditions acting on the body which 
contains and nourishes the germ-plasm can have 
effects which are transmitted unless the germ-plasm 
itself is altered. 

Weismann in a series of books and papers has 
built up a detailed and highly complicated and 
speculative scheme of the nature and composition 
of the germ-plasm, only a brief summary of which 
can be given here. Much of it will doubtless not 
stand the test of fuller investigation, and parts of 
it are already discredited ; but it has had the merit 
of stimulating an immense amount of valuable re- 
search, and there are indications that some of his 
fundamental ideas will form the foundations of a 
true theory of the material basis of heredity. 

Weismann assumes that the germ-plasm is con- 
tained in the nucleus of the cell, and, in particular, 
in the bodies known as chromosomes. [very nucleus 
contains a number of these bodies, in the ordinary 
condition of the nucleus distributed through its 
substance so as to be unrecognisable, but when the 
cell is about to divide they make their appearance 
as rod-like bodies whose number in general is con- 
stant in the nuclei of the same species of animal or 


122 HEREDITY [ AP. 


plant. Before the nucleus divides the chromosomes 
split longitudinally so that they are accurately halved 
and the two halves of each go into different daughter- 
nuclei. Weismann supposes that the germ-plasm 
is contained in the chromosomes, and consists of 
numerous units with different properties. When the 
chromosome splits, each unit is supposed to divide 
into two similar halves, and thus each daughter- 
nucleus receives a similar complement of germ-plasm. 

In the union of male and female cells in fertilisa- 
tion, the nucleus of each cell brings its complement 
of chromosomes, and thus if there were no special 
provision, the number of chromosomes would be 
doubled in each generation. But it is actually found 
that in the cell-divisions immediately preceding the 
development of both male and female sex-cells, a 
process occurs which results in the removal of half 
the chromosomes from the nucleus, and thus when 
the male and female nuclei unite the normal number 
of chromosomes for the species is restored. Since 
Weismann regards the chromosomes as consisting 
of germ-plasm, and as made up of a vast number 
of units, each of which is the determinant for one 
hereditary character, he saw that, without some such 
process of removal of chromosomes in the formation 
of the sex-cells, the germ-plasm must in a few genera- 
tions become infinitely complicated. He therefore 
predicted that some process of ‘reduction’ of chromo- 


1] HEREDITY 123 


somes must occur, either by elimination of complete 
chromosomes or by transverse instead of longitudinal 
splitting, before any complete observations had been 
made showing that this actually happens. 

Since Weismann supposes that the germ-plasm 
is contained in the chromosomes of the germ-cells, 
and since half the chromosomes are removed in the 
‘maturation’ of these cells without preventing the 
transmission of any part to the offspring by inherit- 
ance, he concluded that each chromosome contains 
all the units (‘determinants ’) necessary to a complete 
individual. (Later work has rendered this conclusion 
doubtful: see Appendix 11.) When fusion of male 
and female sex-cellstakes place, theresulting individual 
will contain a mixture of the parental germ-plasms, 
the paternal in some chromosomes, the maternal in 
others. In the maturation of the sex-cells half 
these germ-plasms will be removed and in the next 
generation a fresh mixture will take place. It thus 
follows that the different chromosomes contain germ- 
plasms descended from different ancestors, and 
different mixtures of these will occur in different 
individuals. Here then we come to Weismann’s 
hypothesis of the origin of variation. Since different 
individuals contain different combinations of an- 
cestral germ-plasms, these will lead to varying 
effects in the development of the body ; new com- 
binations will be continually occurring in every 


124 HEREDITY [ AP. 


fertilised egg, and thus arises the variation between 
separate individuals. Further, although by his theory 
changes brought about in the body-plasm cannot be 
transferred to the germ-plasm, yet influences acting 
on the germ-plasm itself may modify it and so their 
effects will be transmitted. The most important of 
these influences is nourishment, which may favour 
some units of the germ-plasm rather than others. 
He further supposes that there may be competition 
for nourishment among the different units (‘deter- 
minants’) so that some increase at the expense of 
others, and if this process should be continued 
through a series of generations, certain characters 
would show a steady increase while others corre- 
spondingly decrease. Variation thus arises by changes 
brought about in the germ-plasm, and by the 
recombination of varied ancestral germ-plasms in 
each generation. Such variations will be inherited, 
and in this respect will differ entirely from changes 
brought about in the body during its life by the 
action of environment. 

It has been shown that in the earlier theories 
of heredity it was assumed that the germ-cells were 
produced by the body, and that they must therefore 
be supposed either to contain samples of all parts 
of it, or at least some kind of units derived from 
those parts and able to cause their development 
in the next generation. Gradually, as the study 


1| HEREDITY 125 


of heredity and of the actual origin of the germ-cells 
has progressed, biologists have given up this view 
in favour of a belief in germinal continuity, that is, 
that the germ-substance is derived from previous 
germ-substance, the body being a kind of offshoot 
from it. The child is thus like its parent, not 
because it is produced from the parent, but because 
both child and parent are produced from the same 
stock of germ-plasm. 


APPENDIX II 
THE MATERIAL BASIS OF INHERITANCE 


In Appendix 1, it has been mentioned that 
Weismann regarded the chromosomes of the nucleus 
as the bearers of hereditary characters, and more 
recent work has added to the probability of this 
view, while not as yet providing anything which can 
be called proof. That some, if not all, the hereditary 
characters are determined by the nucleus of the 
germ-cell is indicated by several facts. In the first 
place, the spermatozoon consists of little else but a 
nucleus with a vibrating tail, and the tail may be 
shed as the spermatozoon enters the ovum. Secondly, 
experiments in fertilising non-nucleated fragments 
of sea-urchin eggs by sperm of a different species, 
give evidence that the hereditary characters of the 
resulting larvae are exclusively those of the paternal 
species. This conclusion however has been disputed, 
and can only be regarded as probable rather than 
certain. Again, experiments in fertilising one egg 
simultaneously by more than one spermatozoon, lead 


Ae 1K HEREDITY 127 


Boveri [3] to believe that the subsequent develop- 
ment of the cells of the embryo depends on the 
distribution of the chromosomes in the abnormal 
divisions consequent on double fertilisation. And 
Herbst [16] has obtained sea-urchin larvae made by 
crossing distinct species, which on one side of the 
body resemble one parent, and on the other side 
the other parent. He shows that these differences 
are connected with differences in the size of the 
nuclei of the two sides, and that probably the part 
with maternal characters contains only maternal 
nuclear substance, while the part showing the 
paternal character has nuclei derived from both 
parents. 

But probably the best evidence for regarding the 
chromosomes as bearing the essential determinants 
for hereditary characters is provided by the behaviour 
of the chromosomes themselves in the maturation 
divisions of the germ-cells. It has been pointed out 
that at these nuclear divisions the chromosome 
number is halved, and restored to the full number 
again in the next generation by the union of two 
germ-cells each bearing the half-number. Now it 
has been found in certain cases that the chromosomes 
are not all alike, but differ among themselves in size 
and shape, and when this is so it can be seen that 
the nucleus just before maturation contains two of 
each kind. If the different kinds of chromosomes 


128 HEREDITY [ AP. 


are represented by letters, A, B, C, D..., there will 
then be two A’s, two B’s, etc. in the nucleus. The 
actual processes in the reduction division are some- 
what complex, but briefly they consist in a pairing 
together of the chromosomes, followed by a division 
of the nucleus in which the two members of each 
pair are separated into different daughter-nuclei, 
so that the daughter-nuclei each contain half the 
full number. When the chromosomes differ among 
themselves, it is seen that two similar ones always 
pair together, le. A with A, B with B, ete. Thus 
the daughter-nuclei each contain the whole series 
A, B, C..., but have only one of each, instead of two. 

If then it is Imagined that each chromosome is 
the bearer of the determinant (or ‘factor’) for a 
Mendelian character, we may regard one individual 
as having a double series of chromosomes A, B, C..., 
etc., and another as bearing the allelomorphic cha- 
racters a, b, ¢..., etc. When these individuals are 
mated, the heterozygote will bear both series, 
A and a, B and J, etc. In the formation of the 
germ-cells, A will segregate from a, B from 6 in 
exactly the way required by Mendelian theory. But 
there is no reason to suppose the series A, B, C... 
should all go into one germ-cell, and a, 6, ¢... into the 
other ; A may go into the first daughter-nucleus and 
a into the second, but 6 may go with A into the first, 
and B into the second. So in crossing races differmg 


11] HEREDITY 129 


in more than one allelomorphic pair, all possible 
combinations can be produced, except that no germ- 
cell can contain both the members of one pair. 

The suggestion that this segregation of chromo- 
somes, which can be seen to take place, is the 
mechanism by which the members of an allelo- 
morphic pair of characters are segregated, is quite 
speculative; but it seems exceedingly unlikely that 
machinery so exactly adapted to bring it about should 
be found in every developing germ-cell, if it had no 
connexion with the segregation of characters that 
is observed in experimental breeding. There is also 
the further fact in support of the suggestion, that 
it is known in many insects that one pair of chromo- 
somes is closely connected with sex, for in the males of 
these species one chromosome of the pair is absent or 
much reduced, but in the female both are similar. 
These sex-chromosomes separate from one another 
like the others (when both are present), and it has 
been seen that there is experimental evidence for the 
view that the sex-determiners behave like Mendelian 
allelomorphs. One serious difficulty however suggests 
itself at once; the chromosomes are limited in number, 
and it is undoubted that more allelomorphic pairs of 
characters may exist in a species than there are pairs 
of chromosomes, although in such cases there is no 
evidence that members of different pairs are always 
associated together. Several suggestions have been 

D. 9 


130 HEREDITY [ AP. 


made to meet this difficulty, of which perhaps the 
most adequate is that the chromosomes are not in- 
divisible entities, but are composed of smaller units, 
each of which corresponds with one Mendelian factor. 
The chromosomes are not permanently present in 
the distinct form which is seen during cell-division, 
but during the resting condition of the nucleus their 
substance becomes diffused over a network of threads, 
only to be collected again into definite chromosomes, 
having the same number and form as before, pre- 
paratory to the next division. If each chromosome 
consists of a series of units having a definite arrange- 
ment, and these units become scattered in the 
‘resting phase, but are re-collected in the same 
order when the chromosomes are re-formed, it does 
not seem unlikely that a unit NV may take the place 
of the corresponding unit ” from the other chromo- 
some of the pair, so that if the chromosome A 
consisted at one division of units W/, N, O..., and the 
corresponding chromosome @ consisted of m, 7, 0..., 
after the resting stage V and x might have exchanged 
places, and chromosome A would consist of W, n, O... 
and a of m, N, o..... By some process of this kind 
it seems probable that the observed phenomena of 
chromosome reduction would account for all the 
facts of Mendelian segregation. 

It must be stated quite clearly, however, that 
the study of the possible relation between chromo- 


I] HEREDITY 131 


somes and body-characters is as yet in its Infancy ; 
and this brief note can only sketch the lines on which 
modern work seems to support Weismann’s hypo- 
thesis that the chromosomes are the physical basis 
of inheritance. It will be seen that his suggestion 
that all the chromosomes are on the whole similar 
is not confirmed, but the evidence that chromosomes 
do bear factors for inherited characters is consider- 
ably stronger than when the idea was first put 
forward. 


Or 


iN 


LITERATURE LIST 


The Works marked * are general treatises suitable 
Sor further study. 


Bateson, W. Materials for the Study of Variation. London, 
1894. 

—- Mendel’ Principles of Heredity. 2nd Impression. 
Cambridge, 1909. (Full bibliography to date of publication.) 

Boveri, T. Ergebnisse tiber die Konstitution der Chroma- 
tischen Substanz des Zellkerns. Jena, 1904. 

Castle, W. E. Heredity of Hair-length in Guinea-pigs, and 
its Bearing on the Theory of Pure Gametes. Publ. Carnegie 
Inst. Washington, No. 49, 1906. 

Studies of Inheritance in Rabbits. Publ. Carnegie 
Inst. No. 114, 1909. 

Darwin, C. The Origin of Species. 

—— Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication. 

Darwin and Modern Science. Cambridge, 1909. 

Davenport, C.B. Statistical Methods with Special Reference 
to Biological Variation. New York and London, 1899. 

Eugenics Laboratory. Publications of the Galton Laboratory 
for National Eugenics, University of London :— 

Especially, Elderton, E. M. Relative strength of Nature 
and Nurture. Heron, D. A First Study of the Statistics 
of Insanity. Schuster, E. Inheritance of Ability. 

Ewart, J.C. The Penycuik Experiments. London, 1899. 


eal 


26. 


Palle 


LITERATURE LIST 133 


Galton, F. Inquiries into Human Faculty and its Develop- 
ment. Macmillan, 1883 (Cheap Edition, J. M. Dent). 

— Natural Inheritance. Macmillan, 1889. 

Essays in Eugenics. Eugenics Education Soc. Lon- 

don, 1909. 

Hereditary Genius. Macmillan, 1869. 

Herbst, C. Vererbungsstudien, 1—vi. Archiv fiir Entwick- 
lungsmechanik, 1906—1909. Especially v—v1, 1907 and 
1909. 

Hurst, C. C. On the Inheritance of Eye-colour in Man. 
Proc. Roy. Soc. (B) Vol. 80. 1908. 

Johannsen, W. Ueber Erblichkeit in Populationen und 
in Reinen Linien. Jena, 1903. 

Kammerer, P. Vererbung erzwungener Fortpflanzungsan- 
passungen. Arch. f. Entwicklungsmechanik, Vols. xxv 
(1907) and xxvii (1909). 

Kellogg, V. L. Darwinism Today. London and New York, 
1907. 

Lock, R. H. Recent Progress in the Study of Variation, 
Heredity and Evolution. 2nd Edition. Murray, 1909. 

Lotzy, J. P. Vorlesungen iber Descendenztheorien. Jena, 
1906. 

Nettleship, E. Bowman Lecture. Trans. Ophthalm. Soc. 
1909. 

Pearson, K. The Grammar of Science. 2nd Edition. 
London, 1900. 

Mathematical Contributions to the Theory of Evolution. 
Proce. and Trans. Roy. Soc. (A) 1896—1903; also numerous 
papers in Biometrika, 1902— , especially ‘On the Laws 
of Inheritance in Man,’ 1903, 1904; and ‘The Law of 
Ancestral Heredity,’ 1903. 

—— Huxley Lecture of Anthropological Inst. of Gt Britain 
and Ireland. Trans. Anthrop. Inst. 1903, p. 179. 

Punnett, R.C. Mendelism. 2nd Edition. Cambridge, 1907. 


154 


728. 
a9! 


¥43. 


44. 


LITERATURE LIST 


Reid, Archdall. The Principles of Heredity. London, 1906. 

Romanes, J.G. Darwin and After Darwin. 3 vols. London, 
1892—1897. 

Royal Society. Reports to the*Evolution Committee, I—v, 
1902—1909. 

Schuster, E. Hereditary Deafness. Biometrika, rv, 1906, 
p. 465. 

Standfuss. Handbuch der Paliarctischen Grossschmetter- 
linge. Jena, 1896. 

Thomson, J. A. Heredity. Murray 1908. (Very good 
bibliography.) 

Tower. An Investigation of Evolution in Chrysomelid 
Beetles of the Genus Leptinotarsa. Publ. Carnegie Inst. 
Washington, No. 48, 1906. 

Treasury of Human Inheritance. Publ. Galton Laboratory 
for Nat. Eugenics, London University. 

Vries, H. de. The Mutation Theory (Trans. ). London, 1910. 

— Species and Varieties, their origin through Mutation. 
Chicago and London, 1905. 

Wallace, A. R. Darwinism. London, 1889. 

Weismann, A. Essays upon Heredity and Kindred Subjects 
(Trans.). Oxford, 1891, 1892. 

— The Germ Plasm. (Trans. Parker and Rénnfeldt). 
London, 1893. 

The Evolution Theory (Trans. J. A. and M. R. 
Thomson). London, 1904. 

Wheldale, Muriel. Plant Oxydases and the Chemical Inter- 
relationships of Colour-varieties. Progressus Ee. Botani- 
cae, I, p. 457. Jena, 1910. 

Whetham, W.C. D. and C. The Family and the Nation. 
London, 1910. 

Yule, G. U. Mendel’s Laws and their probable relations 
to intra-racial Heredity. New Phytologist. Vol. 1, 1902, 
p. 195. 


GLOSSARY 


Acquired Character. A feature developed during the life of the 
individual possessing it, in response to the action of use or 
environment. 

Albino. An animal without pigment in the skin, hair or eyes. 
The hair is white; the eyes pink owing to the colour of the 
blood. Among plants, white-flowered varieties are called 
albinos. The condition is called albinism. 

Allelomorph. One of a pair of alternative Mendelian characters. 
When a pair of characters are alternative in their in- 
heritance, and segregate from each other in the formation of 
the germ-cells of an individual which contains both, they are 
said to be allelomorphic with each other. See Segregation. 

Anther. The part of the stamen in a flower which contains the 
pollen. 

Chromogen. A colourless substance which when oxidised gives 
rise to a coloured body (pigment). 

Chromosome. A body inthe nucleus of a cell, which absorbs staims 
and becomes clearly visible during nuclear division, but be- 
comes dispersed through the nucleus during the resting phase. 
During nuclear division each chromosome becomes accurately 
halved, so that in general all cells of each species of animal 
or plant contain an equal number of chromosomes. 

Continuous. See Variation. 

Determinant. The hypothetical unit in a germ-cell which 
determines the production of a particular character in the 
individual derived from that germ-cell. See Factor. 


136 GLOSSARY 


Deviation. The amount by which an individual differs from the 
mode in continuous variation. 

Dimorphism. The condition in which a species exists in two 
distinct types or sharply separable varieties. When the two 
sexes differ thus, the condition is called Sexwal Dimorphism. 

Dominant. When two varieties, differing in one character, are 
crossed together, and all the offspring have the character 
borne by one parent, that character is dominant. Applied 
to one of a pair of Mendelian allelomorphs. 

Egg-cell. The germ-cell produced by the female. 

Lpistatic. When one character 4 is superposed upon another B, 
so that A prevents or obscures the appearance of JB, al- 
though they are not allelomorphic with each other, A is said 
to be epistatic to B. 

‘Extracted’ Homozygote. When two heterozygous individuals 
are mated together, their homozygous offspring are spoken of 
as ‘extracted’ homozygotes. 

F,, F,. The Symbol F; is used to indicate the offspring (first 
filial generation) of a mating between two differing individuals. 
The later generations (second, third filial, etc.) are repre- 
sented by #2, F3, ete. 

Factor. In Mendelian inheritance, the hereditary determinant 
(q.v.) of a particular character is spoken of as the jactor for 
that character. 

Ferment. A body which has the power of causing chemical 
action between substances which in its absence are inactive 
towards one another. 

Fertilisation. The union of male and female germ-cells which 
precedes the development of a new individual. It consists 
essentially in the fusion of the nuclei of the germ-cells. 

Gamete. A germ-cell, q.v. 

Gemmules. Wypothetical bodies supposed to be given off by the 
cells of the body, and entering the germ-cells, to transmit 
heritable characters to the next generation. 


GLOSSARY 137 


Germ-cell. A reproductive cell, which, usually after union with 
a germ-cell from another individual (fertilisation), develops 
into a new individual. In animals the germ-cells of the male 
are spermatozoa, those of the female ora (egg-cells). In 
plants the male germ-cells are contained in the pollen; the 
female, egg-cells, in the ovules or embryo-seeds. 

Germ-plasm. The germinal substance, which according to 
Weismann is alone able to give origin to new individuals. 

Heterozygote. An individual containing both members of an 
allelomorphic pair of characters, i.e. which is hybrid in respect 
of that pair of characters, and produces germ-cells bearing 
one and the other respectively. Adjective—heterozygous. 

Homozygote. An individual made by union of two germ-cells 
each of which bears the same member of an allelomorphic 
pair of characters, so that it is ‘pure’ in respect of that 
character, and all its germ-cells bear the same character. 
Adjective—homozygous. 

Mode. The most frequent condition of a character which varies 
continuously. Its measurement is called the modal value. 

Mutation. A variety which is not connected with the type by 
intermediates. More strictly, the sudden origin of such a 
variety. 

Nucleus. A sharply defined body found in every cell, which seems 
to control the activities of the cell. 

Ovum. The germ-cell produced by the female, an egg-cell. 

Pin-eye. In Primula (Primrose, Cowslip, etc.), the form in 
which the style is long and the anthers low down in the 
flower-tube. The other form, with short style and anthers 
high up, is called Thrum-eye. 

Pollen. The powder bearing the male germ-cells in a flowering 
plant. 

Polymorphism. The condition in which a species exists in 
several distinct forms or varieties. 

Recessive. When two individuals are crossed, bearing different 


138 GLOSSARY 


members of an allelomorphic pair of characters, the member 
of the pair which does not appear in the offspring is called 
recessive. 

Reversion. A ‘throw-back’ to a previous ancestor, or to the 
type of the species, when varieties are crossed. 

Segregation. In Mendelian inheritance, the separation of the 
two characters of an allelomorphic pair, in a heterozygote, 
into distinct germ-cells, i.e. the formation of gametes each 
bearing one character of a pair, by an individual which 
contains both members. 

Self-fertilisation. The fertilisation of a female gamete by a male 
gamete produced from the same individual. The process in 
plants is spoken of shortly as selfing. 

Spermatozoon. See Germ-cell. 

Style. The part of a flower which receives the pollen, and con- 
ducts the male germ-cell to the egg-cell. 

Telegony. The supposed influence of a former sire upon young 
born to a later sire by the same mother. 

Type. The normal form of a species, which is regarded as 
typical. 

Variation, Variability. The differing among themselves of 
individuals of the same species. When the extreme forms 
are connected by a complete series of intermediates, the 
variation is Continuous; when distinct forms occur, not 
connected by intermediates, it is Discontinuous. 

Zygote. An individual produced by the union of two gametes. 


INDEX 


Ability, Inheritance of, 48 
Acquired Characters, 19, 24, 30, 

45, 90-97, 117, 121 
Albinism, 66, 71 
Allelomorph, 56, 61, 71, 128 
Amphidasys betularia, 89 
Ancestral Heredity, Law of, 42 
Andalusian Fowl, 69 
Bateson, W., 19, 70, 76, 82 
Bee, 93 
Birren, R. H., 64 
Biometric Methods 9, 32-51 
Boveri, T., 127 
Brachydactyly, 103 
BRowN-SEQUARD, 94 
Cataract, 104 
Chromosomes, 121, 126-131 
Colour-blindness, 84 
Colour-inheritance, 71-83 
Combs of Fowls, 66, 67 
Correlation, 36 

a parental, 39, 101 

CunnincHam, J. T., 91 
Darwin, 2, 8, 23, 24, 88, 117 
Deafness, 101 
Dihybridism, 62 
Disease, Inheritance of, 100 
Dominant Characters, 54 
Earwig, 17 
Environment, 24-29, 110-115 
Epistatic, 73, 75 
Eugenics, 51 
Evolution, 2, 88, 117 


Ewart, J.C., 96 

Extracted Homozygote, 62, 86 
Kye-colour, 66, 102, 107 
Feeble-minded, 50 

Flatfish, 91 

Flower-colour, 66, 74, 77, 79-83 
Fluctuation, 22, 45 

Fowls, 66, 67 

Gatton, Sir F., 24, 30, 33, 41, 111 
Gametes, 57 (see Germ-cells) 
Gemmules, 118 

Germ-cells, 57, 118, 120, 126 
Germinal Continuity, 119, 125 
Germ-plasm, 20, 23-26, 120-125 
Gregory, R. P., 82 
Guinea-pig, 86, 94 
Hair-colour, 66, 107 
Hair-length, 66, 86 

Hersest, C., 127 

Heron, D., 101 

Heterozygote, 59 

Homostyle (Primula), 82 
Homozygote, 61 

Horns of Sheep, 84 

Horst, C. C., 102, 107 
Hybridisation, 53 

Insanity, 100, 112 

Instinct, 92 

JOHANNSEN, W., 44, 87 
KamMERER, P., 95 

Lamarck, 117 

Leptinotarsa, 28 

Lychnis, 66 


140 


Maize, 65, 66 

Man, Heredity in, 99-115 
Marsh-marigold, 15 
Maternal Impression, 97 
Mendelian Heredity, 52-89 

as i in Man, 100, 
102-107 
Mendel’s Experiments, 53, 54 
Mental and Moral Characters, 49 
Mid-parent, 40 

Mode, 10, 34 

Mouse, 74 

Mulatto, 108 

Mutation, 22 

Natural Selection, 8, 89, 117 
NettTuesHip, K., 104, 106 
Night-blindness, 104 
Nucleus, 121, 126 

Organic Stability, 24 
Pangenesis, 118 

Prarson, K., 32, 41 

Peas, 54 

Pigeons, 66 

Primula, 66, 79, 81-83 
Protoplasm, 3 

Punnett, KR. C., 70 

‘Pure Lines,’ 44, 87 

Rabbit, 74, 79, 87 

Rat, 72, 79 

Recessive Characters, 56 
Regression, 35 

Retinitis pigmentosa, 105 
Reversion, 74, 76 

Rust in Wheat, 64 
ScuusteErR, E., 49, 101 
Sea-urchin eggs, 126, 127 
Seed-colour, 66 


INDEX 


Segregation, 58 

Sex, 84, 103, 106 

Sheep, 84 

Silkworms, 94 

Skew Curves, 15 
» Correlation, 39 

Skin-colour, 108 

Social Problems, 112 

STANDFuss, M., 27 

Statistical Study of Variation, 32 

Stature, 10, 34, 37 

Sterility, 66, 110 

Stocks, 66, 80 

Sweet-pea, 54, 66, 76 

Telegony, 96 

Temperature, effects of, 25, 27, 28 

Tortoiseshell Cat, 84 

Tower, W. L., 28 

Tuberculosis, 25 

Twins, 30, 111 

Variation, 2, 5, 7 

= Causes of, 8, 23-31, 

123 

Variation, Continuous and Dis- 
continuous, 9-12, 17-19, 22, 88 

Variation, Curves illustrating, 
11-17 

Variation, induced by crossing, 
2 

Vries, H. de, 19 

Watiace, A. R., 8 

Weismann, A., 20, 23, 119-125 

We.pon, W. F. R., 33 

Wheat, 64, 66 

WHELDALE, MurtieL, 77 

Yuus, -G. U., 42 

Zygote, 58 


CAMBRIDGE: PRINTED BY JOHN CLAY, M.A. AT THE UNIVERSITY PRESS 


f 


Doncas te! 


a ——_— 


fo Rest ks 


fi 


tit] 
aie 
hee 


SMITHSONIAN INSTITUTION LIBRARIES 


MT Mil 


| 


Spey eenece te T 
| 


| 
| 


| ! ] 


sstiasestes 


Ss cS. 
Sa peere ete et ie eee se ea 
peetee Sct te Sees: =e ~eaaste 
SSS 


Stetedtres tes tees 


roueregeceoss 


sesesersetecttte tee tesa eet 


joe e se eer ee eens 


Ss SSS SS Sie carci weave Se Sgereepe ey ees 
SS SS SSS SSS 

sraaeee es = SS = = 

patos siesee = 

paeeose es a SS 


ee eeaeecs 
Drs Se Se 


SSS HSS 


title 


attract 
ernie ity 


j 
1) 
tr 


ne 


ichpede 


“ 


2023232328 Se = 
Bee peat ss tie here te ces eee eee 


yjdaleites 


SS SS SSS SS sors seaete ese s cose 


stake ete